词条 | Memory |
释义 |
Memory is the faculty of the brain by which information is encoded, stored, and retrieved when needed. Memory is vital to experiences, it is the retention of information over time for the purpose of influencing future action.[1] If we could not remember past events, we could not learn or develop language, relationships, or personal identity.[2] Often memory is understood as an informational processing system with explicit and implicit functioning that is made up of a sensory processor, short-term (or working) memory, and long-term memory.[3] This can be related to the neuron. The sensory processor allows information from the outside world to be sensed in the form of chemical and physical stimuli and attended to various levels of focus and intent. Working memory serves as an encoding and retrieval processor. Information in the form of stimuli is encoded in accordance with explicit or implicit functions by the working memory processor. The working memory also retrieves information from previously stored material. Finally, the function of long-term memory is to store data through various categorical models or systems.[3] Explicit and implicit functions of memory are also known as declarative and non-declarative systems.[4][5] These systems involve the purposeful intention of memory retrieval and storage, or lack thereof. Declarative, or explicit, memory is the conscious storage and recollection of data.[6] Under declarative memory resides semantic and episodic memory. Semantic memory refers to memory that is encoded with specific meaning [2], while episodic memory refers to information that is encoded along a spatial and temporal plane.[7][8][9] Declarative memory is usually the primary process thought of when referencing memory.[2]Non-declarative, or implicit, memory is the unconscious storage and recollection of information.[13] An example of a non-declarative process would be the unconscious learning or retrieval of information by way of procedural memory, or a priming phenomenon.[2] [13][16] Priming is the process of subliminally arousing specific responses from memory and shows that not all memory is consciously activated,[10] whereas procedural memory is the slow and gradual learning of skills that often occurs without conscious attention to learning.[2][11]Memory is not a perfect processor, and is affected by many factors. The ways by which information is encoded, stored, and retrieved can all be corrupted. The amount of attention given new stimuli can diminish the amount of information that becomes encoded for storage.[2] Also, the storage process can become corrupted by physical damage to areas of the brain that are associated with memory storage, such as the hippocampus.[12][13] Finally, the retrieval of information from long-term memory can be disrupted because of decay within long-term memory.[2] Normal functioning, decay over time, and brain damage all affect the accuracy and capacity of the memory.[14][15] Memory loss is usually described as forgetfulness or amnesia.[16][17][18][19][20][21] Sensory memory{{Main|Sensory memory}}Sensory memory holds sensory information less than one second after an item is perceived. The ability to look at an item and remember what it looked like with just a split second of observation, or memorization, is the example of sensory memory. It is out of cognitive control and is an automatic response. With very short presentations, participants often report that they seem to "see" more than they can actually report. The first experiments exploring this form of sensory memory were precisely conducted by George Sperling (1963)[22] using the "partial report paradigm". Subjects were presented with a grid of 12 letters, arranged into three rows of four. After a brief presentation, subjects were then played either a high, medium or low tone, cuing them which of the rows to report. Based on these partial report experiments, Sperling was able to show that the capacity of sensory memory was approximately 12 items, but that it degraded very quickly (within a few hundred milliseconds). Because this form of memory degrades so quickly, participants would see the display but be unable to report all of the items (12 in the "whole report" procedure) before they decayed. This type of memory cannot be prolonged via rehearsal. Three types of sensory memories exist. Iconic memory is a fast decaying store of visual information; a type of sensory memory that briefly stores an image which has been perceived for a small duration. Echoic memory is a fast decaying store of auditory information, another type of sensory memory that briefly stores sounds that have been perceived for short durations.[23] Haptic memory is a type of sensory memory that represents a database for touch stimuli. Short-term memory{{Main|Short-term memory}}Short-term memory is also known as working memory. Short-term memory allows recall for a period of several seconds to a minute without rehearsal. Its capacity is also very limited: George A. Miller (1956), when working at Bell Laboratories, conducted experiments showing that the store of short-term memory was 7±2 items (the title of his famous paper, "The magical number 7±2"). Modern estimates of the capacity of short-term memory are lower, typically of the order of 4–5 items;[24] however, memory capacity can be increased through a process called chunking.[25] For example, in recalling a ten-digit telephone number, a person could chunk the digits into three groups: first, the area code (such as 123), then a three-digit chunk (456) and lastly a four-digit chunk (7890). This method of remembering telephone numbers is far more effective than attempting to remember a string of 10 digits; this is because we are able to chunk the information into meaningful groups of numbers. This may be reflected in some countries in the tendency to display telephone numbers as several chunks of two to four numbers. Short-term memory is believed to rely mostly on an acoustic code for storing information, and to a lesser extent a visual code. Conrad (1964)[26] found that test subjects had more difficulty recalling collections of letters that were acoustically similar (e.g. E, P, D). Confusion with recalling acoustically similar letters rather than visually similar letters implies that the letters were encoded acoustically. Conrad's (1964) study, however, deals with the encoding of written text; thus, while memory of written language may rely on acoustic components, generalisations to all forms of memory cannot be made. Long-term memory{{Main|Long-term memory}}The storage in sensory memory and short-term memory generally has a strictly limited capacity and duration, which means that information is not retained indefinitely. By contrast, long-term memory can store much larger quantities of information for potentially unlimited duration (sometimes a whole life span). Its capacity is immeasurable. For example, given a random seven-digit number we may remember it for only a few seconds before forgetting, suggesting it was stored in our short-term memory. On the other hand, we can remember telephone numbers for many years through repetition; this information is said to be stored in long-term memory. While short-term memory encodes information acoustically, long-term memory encodes it semantically: Baddeley (1966)[27] discovered that, after 20 minutes, test subjects had the most difficulty recalling a collection of words that had similar meanings (e.g. big, large, great, huge) long-term. Another part of long-term memory is episodic memory, "which attempts to capture information such as 'what', 'when' and 'where{{'"}}.[28] With episodic memory, individuals are able to recall specific events such as birthday parties and weddings. Short-term memory is supported by transient patterns of neuronal communication, dependent on regions of the frontal lobe (especially dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and the parietal lobe. Long-term memory, on the other hand, is maintained by more stable and permanent changes in neural connections widely spread throughout the brain. The hippocampus is essential (for learning new information) to the consolidation of information from short-term to long-term memory, although it does not seem to store information itself. It was thought that without the hippocampus new memories were unable to be stored into long-term memory and that there would be a very short attention span, as first gleaned from patient Henry Molaison[29] after what was thought to be the full removal of both his hippocampi. More recent examination of his brain, post-mortem, shows that the hippocampus was more intact than first thought, throwing theories drawn from the initial data into question. The hippocampus may be involved in changing neural connections for a period of three months or more after the initial learning. Research has suggested that long-term memory storage in humans may be maintained by DNA methylation,[30] and the 'prion' gene.[31][32] Multi-store model{{See also|Memory consolidation}}The multi-store model (also known as Atkinson–Shiffrin memory model) was first described in 1968 by Atkinson and Shiffrin. The multi-store model has been criticised for being too simplistic. For instance, long-term memory is believed to be actually made up of multiple subcomponents, such as episodic and procedural memory. It also proposes that rehearsal is the only mechanism by which information eventually reaches long-term storage, but evidence shows us capable of remembering things without rehearsal. The model also shows all the memory stores as being a single unit whereas research into this shows differently. For example, short-term memory can be broken up into different units such as visual information and acoustic information. In a study by Zlonoga and Gerber (1986), patient 'KF' demonstrated certain deviations from the Atkinson–Shiffrin model. Patient KF was brain damaged, displaying difficulties regarding short-term memory. Recognition of sounds such as spoken numbers, letters, words and easily identifiable noises (such as doorbells and cats meowing) were all impacted. Visual short-term memory was unaffected, suggesting a dichotomy between visual and audial memory.[33] Working memory{{Main|Working memory}}In 1974 Baddeley and Hitch proposed a "working memory model" that replaced the general concept of short-term memory with an active maintenance of information in the short-term storage. In this model, working memory consists of three basic stores: the central executive, the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. In 2000 this model was expanded with the multimodal episodic buffer (Baddeley's model of working memory).[34] The central executive essentially acts as an attention sensory store. It channels information to the three component processes: the phonological loop, the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and the episodic buffer. The phonological loop stores auditory information by silently rehearsing sounds or words in a continuous loop: the articulatory process (for example the repetition of a telephone number over and over again). A short list of data is easier to remember. The visuospatial sketchpad stores visual and spatial information. It is engaged when performing spatial tasks (such as judging distances) or visual ones (such as counting the windows on a house or imagining images). The episodic buffer is dedicated to linking information across domains to form integrated units of visual, spatial, and verbal information and chronological ordering (e.g., the memory of a story or a movie scene). The episodic buffer is also assumed to have links to long-term memory and semantical meaning. The working memory model explains many practical observations, such as why it is easier to do two different tasks (one verbal and one visual) than two similar tasks (e.g., two visual), and the aforementioned word-length effect. Working memory is also the premise for what allows us to do everyday activities involving thought. It is the section of memory where we carry out thought processes and use them to learn and reason about topics.[34] TypesResearchers distinguish between recognition and recall memory. Recognition memory tasks require individuals to indicate whether they have encountered a stimulus (such as a picture or a word) before. Recall memory tasks require participants to retrieve previously learned information. For example, individuals might be asked to produce a series of actions they have seen before or to say a list of words they have heard before. By information typeTopographic memory involves the ability to orient oneself in space, to recognize and follow an itinerary, or to recognize familiar places.[35] Getting lost when traveling alone is an example of the failure of topographic memory.[36]Flashbulb memories are clear episodic memories of unique and highly emotional events.[37] People remembering where they were or what they were doing when they first heard the news of President Kennedy's assassination,[38] the Sydney Siege or of 9/11 are examples of flashbulb memories. Anderson (1976)[39] divides long-term memory into declarative (explicit) and procedural (implicit) memories. Declarative{{Main|Declarative memory}}Declarative memory requires conscious recall, in that some conscious process must call back the information. It is sometimes called explicit memory, since it consists of information that is explicitly stored and retrieved. Declarative memory can be further sub-divided into semantic memory, concerning principles and facts taken independent of context; and episodic memory, concerning information specific to a particular context, such as a time and place. Semantic memory allows the encoding of abstract knowledge about the world, such as "Paris is the capital of France". Episodic memory, on the other hand, is used for more personal memories, such as the sensations, emotions, and personal associations of a particular place or time. Episodic memories often reflect the "firsts" in life such as a first kiss, first day of school or first time winning a championship. These are key events in one's life that can be remembered clearly. Autobiographical memory – memory for particular events within one's own life – is generally viewed as either equivalent to, or a subset of, episodic memory. Visual memory is part of memory preserving some characteristics of our senses pertaining to visual experience. One is able to place in memory information that resembles objects, places, animals or people in sort of a mental image. Visual memory can result in priming and it is assumed some kind of perceptual representational system underlies this phenomenon.{{citation needed|date=November 2013}} ProceduralIn contrast, procedural memory (or implicit memory) is not based on the conscious recall of information, but on implicit learning. It can best be summarized as remembering how to do something. Procedural memory is primarily employed in learning motor skills and should be considered a subset of implicit memory. It is revealed when one does better in a given task due only to repetition – no new explicit memories have been formed, but one is unconsciously accessing aspects of those previous experiences. Procedural memory involved in motor learning depends on the cerebellum and basal ganglia. A characteristic of procedural memory is that the things remembered are automatically translated into actions, and thus sometimes difficult to describe. Some examples of procedural memory include the ability to ride a bike or tie shoelaces.[40] By temporal directionAnother major way to distinguish different memory functions is whether the content to be remembered is in the past, retrospective memory, or in the future, prospective memory. Thus, retrospective memory as a category includes semantic, episodic and autobiographical memory. In contrast, prospective memory is memory for future intentions, or remembering to remember (Winograd, 1988). Prospective memory can be further broken down into event- and time-based prospective remembering. Time-based prospective memories are triggered by a time-cue, such as going to the doctor (action) at 4pm (cue). Event-based prospective memories are intentions triggered by cues, such as remembering to post a letter (action) after seeing a mailbox (cue). Cues do not need to be related to the action (as the mailbox/letter example), and lists, sticky-notes, knotted handkerchiefs, or string around the finger all exemplify cues that people use as strategies to enhance prospective memory. Study techniquesTo assess infantsInfants do not have the language ability to report on their memories and so verbal reports cannot be used to assess very young children's memory. Throughout the years, however, researchers have adapted and developed a number of measures for assessing both infants' recognition memory and their recall memory. Habituation and operant conditioning techniques have been used to assess infants' recognition memory and the deferred and elicited imitation techniques have been used to assess infants' recall memory. Techniques used to assess infants' recognition memory include the following:
Techniques used to assess infants' recall memory include the following:
To assess older children and adultsResearchers use a variety of tasks to assess older children and adults' memory. Some examples are:
Failures
PhysiologyBrain areas involved in the neuroanatomy of memory such as the hippocampus, the amygdala, the striatum, or the mammillary bodies are thought to be involved in specific types of memory. For example, the hippocampus is believed to be involved in spatial learning and declarative learning, while the amygdala is thought to be involved in emotional memory.[54] Damage to certain areas in patients and animal models and subsequent memory deficits is a primary source of information. However, rather than implicating a specific area, it could be that damage to adjacent areas, or to a pathway traveling through the area is actually responsible for the observed deficit. Further, it is not sufficient to describe memory, and its counterpart, learning, as solely dependent on specific brain regions. Learning and memory are usually attributed to changes in neuronal synapses, thought to be mediated by long-term potentiation and long-term depression. However, this has been questioned on computational as well as neurophysiological grounds by the cognitive scientist Charles R. Gallistel and others.[55][56][57] In general, the more emotionally charged an event or experience is, the better it is remembered; this phenomenon is known as the memory enhancement effect. Patients with amygdala damage, however, do not show a memory enhancement effect.[58][59] Hebb distinguished between short-term and long-term memory. He postulated that any memory that stayed in short-term storage for a long enough time would be consolidated into a long-term memory. Later research showed this to be false. Research has shown that direct injections of cortisol or epinephrine help the storage of recent experiences. This is also true for stimulation of the amygdala. This proves that excitement enhances memory by the stimulation of hormones that affect the amygdala. Excessive or prolonged stress (with prolonged cortisol) may hurt memory storage. Patients with amygdalar damage are no more likely to remember emotionally charged words than nonemotionally charged ones. The hippocampus is important for explicit memory. The hippocampus is also important for memory consolidation. The hippocampus receives input from different parts of the cortex and sends its output out to different parts of the brain also. The input comes from secondary and tertiary sensory areas that have processed the information a lot already. Hippocampal damage may also cause memory loss and problems with memory storage.[60] This memory loss includes retrograde amnesia which is the loss of memory for events that occurred shortly before the time of brain damage.[53] Cognitive neuroscienceCognitive neuroscientists consider memory as the retention, reactivation, and reconstruction of the experience-independent internal representation. The term of internal representation implies that such definition of memory contains two components: the expression of memory at the behavioral or conscious level, and the underpinning physical neural changes (Dudai 2007). The latter component is also called engram or memory traces (Semon 1904). Some neuroscientists and psychologists mistakenly equate the concept of engram and memory, broadly conceiving all persisting after-effects of experiences as memory; others argue against this notion that memory does not exist until it is revealed in behavior or thought (Moscovitch 2007). One question that is crucial in cognitive neuroscience is how information and mental experiences are coded and represented in the brain. Scientists have gained much knowledge about the neuronal codes from the studies of plasticity, but most of such research has been focused on simple learning in simple neuronal circuits; it is considerably less clear about the neuronal changes involved in more complex examples of memory, particularly declarative memory that requires the storage of facts and events (Byrne 2007). Convergence-divergence zones might be the neural networks where memories are stored and retrieved. Considering that there are several kinds of memory, depending on types of represented knowledge, underlying mechanisms, processes functions and modes of acquisition, it is likely that different brain areas support different memory systems and that they are in mutual relationships in neuronal networks: "components of memory representation are distributed widely across different parts of the brain as mediated by multiple neocortical circuits".[61]
GeneticsStudy of the genetics of human memory is in its infancy. A notable initial success was the association of APOE with memory dysfunction in Alzheimer's Disease. The search for genes associated with normally varying memory continues. One of the first candidates for normal variation in memory is the protein KIBRA,[62] which appears to be associated with the rate at which material is forgotten over a delay period. There has been some evidence that memories are stored in the nucleus of neurons.[63]{{Primary source inline|date=June 2018}} In infancy{{For|the inability of adults to retrieve early memories|Childhood amnesia}}Up until the mid-1980s it was assumed that infants could not encode, retain, and retrieve information.[64] A growing body of research now indicates that infants as young as 6-months can recall information after a 24-hour delay.[65] Furthermore, research has revealed that as infants grow older they can store information for longer periods of time; 6-month-olds can recall information after a 24-hour period, 9-month-olds after up to five weeks, and 20-month-olds after as long as twelve months.[66] In addition, studies have shown that with age, infants can store information faster. Whereas 14-month-olds can recall a three-step sequence after being exposed to it once, 6-month-olds need approximately six exposures in order to be able to remember it.[45][65] Although 6-month-olds can recall information over the short-term, they have difficulty recalling the temporal order of information. It is only by 9 months of age that infants can recall the actions of a two-step sequence in the correct temporal order – that is, recalling step 1 and then step 2.[67][68] In other words, when asked to imitate a two-step action sequence (such as putting a toy car in the base and pushing in the plunger to make the toy roll to the other end), 9-month-olds tend to imitate the actions of the sequence in the correct order (step 1 and then step 2). Younger infants (6-month-olds) can only recall one step of a two-step sequence.[65] Researchers have suggested that these age differences are probably due to the fact that the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus and the frontal components of the neural network are not fully developed at the age of 6-months.[46][69][70] In fact, the term 'infantile amnesia' refers to the phenomenon of accelerated forgetting during infancy. Importantly, infantile amnesia is not unique to humans, and preclinical research (using rodent models) provides insight into the precise neurobiology of this phenomenon. A review of the literature from behavioral neuroscientist Dr Jee Hyun Kim suggests that accelerated forgetting during early life is at least partly due to rapid growth of the brain during this period.[71] Aging{{Main|Memory and aging}}One of the key concerns of older adults is the experience of memory loss, especially as it is one of the hallmark symptoms of Alzheimer's disease. However, memory loss is qualitatively different in normal aging from the kind of memory loss associated with a diagnosis of Alzheimer's (Budson & Price, 2005). Research has revealed that individuals' performance on memory tasks that rely on frontal regions declines with age. Older adults tend to exhibit deficits on tasks that involve knowing the temporal order in which they learned information;[72] source memory tasks that require them to remember the specific circumstances or context in which they learned information;[73] and prospective memory tasks that involve remembering to perform an act at a future time. Older adults can manage their problems with prospective memory by using appointment books, for example. Gene transcription profiles were determined for the human frontal cortex of individuals from age 26 to 106 years. Numerous genes were identified with reduced expression after age 40, and especially after age 70.[74] Genes that play central roles in memory and learning were among those showing the most significant reduction with age. There was also a marked increase in DNA damage, likely oxidative damage, in the promoters of those genes with reduced expression. It was suggested that DNA damage may reduce the expression of selectively vulnerable genes involved in memory and learning.[74] Disorders{{Main|Memory disorder}}Much of the current knowledge of memory has come from studying memory disorders, particularly amnesia. Loss of memory is known as amnesia. Amnesia can result from extensive damage to: (a) the regions of the medial temporal lobe, such as the hippocampus, dentate gyrus, subiculum, amygdala, the parahippocampal, entorhinal, and perirhinal cortices[75] or the (b) midline diencephalic region, specifically the dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus and the mammillary bodies of the hypothalamus.[76] There are many sorts of amnesia, and by studying their different forms, it has become possible to observe apparent defects in individual sub-systems of the brain's memory systems, and thus hypothesize their function in the normally working brain. Other neurological disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease[77] can also affect memory and cognition. Hyperthymesia, or hyperthymesic syndrome, is a disorder that affects an individual's autobiographical memory, essentially meaning that they cannot forget small details that otherwise would not be stored.[78] Korsakoff's syndrome, also known as Korsakoff's psychosis, amnesic-confabulatory syndrome, is an organic brain disease that adversely affects memory by widespread loss or shrinkage of neurons within the prefrontal cortex.[53]While not a disorder, a common temporary failure of word retrieval from memory is the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. Sufferers of Anomic aphasia (also called Nominal aphasia or Anomia), however, do experience the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon on an ongoing basis due to damage to the frontal and parietal lobes of the brain. Influencing factorsInterference can hamper memorization and retrieval. There is retroactive interference, when learning new information makes it harder to recall old information[79] and proactive interference, where prior learning disrupts recall of new information. Although interference can lead to forgetting, it is important to keep in mind that there are situations when old information can facilitate learning of new information. Knowing Latin, for instance, can help an individual learn a related language such as French – this phenomenon is known as positive transfer.[80] StressStress has a significant effect on memory formation and learning. In response to stressful situations, the brain releases hormones and neurotransmitters (ex. glucocorticoids and catecholamines) which affect memory encoding processes in the hippocampus. Behavioural research on animals shows that chronic stress produces adrenal hormones which impact the hippocampal structure in the brains of rats.[81] An experimental study by German cognitive psychologists L. Schwabe and O. Wolf demonstrates how learning under stress also decreases memory recall in humans.[82] In this study, 48 healthy female and male university students participated in either a stress test or a control group. Those randomly assigned to the stress test group had a hand immersed in ice cold water (the reputable SECPT or 'Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test') for up to three minutes, while being monitored and videotaped. Both the stress and control groups were then presented with 32 words to memorize. Twenty-four hours later, both groups were tested to see how many words they could remember (free recall) as well as how many they could recognize from a larger list of words (recognition performance). The results showed a clear impairment of memory performance in the stress test group, who recalled 30% fewer words than the control group. The researchers suggest that stress experienced during learning distracts people by diverting their attention during the memory encoding process. However, memory performance can be enhanced when material is linked to the learning context, even when learning occurs under stress. A separate study by cognitive psychologists Schwabe and Wolf shows that when retention testing is done in a context similar to or congruent with the original learning task (i.e., in the same room), memory impairment and the detrimental effects of stress on learning can be attenuated.[83] Seventy-two healthy female and male university students, randomly assigned to the SECPT stress test or to a control group, were asked to remember the locations of 15 pairs of picture cards – a computerized version of the card game "Concentration" or "Memory". The room in which the experiment took place was infused with the scent of vanilla, as odour is a strong cue for memory. Retention testing took place the following day, either in the same room with the vanilla scent again present, or in a different room without the fragrance. The memory performance of subjects who experienced stress during the object-location task decreased significantly when they were tested in an unfamiliar room without the vanilla scent (an incongruent context); however, the memory performance of stressed subjects showed no impairment when they were tested in the original room with the vanilla scent (a congruent context). All participants in the experiment, both stressed and unstressed, performed faster when the learning and retrieval contexts were similar.[84] This research on the effects of stress on memory may have practical implications for education, for eyewitness testimony and for psychotherapy: students may perform better when tested in their regular classroom rather than an exam room, eyewitnesses may recall details better at the scene of an event than in a courtroom, and persons suffering from post-traumatic stress may improve when helped to situate their memories of a traumatic event in an appropriate context. Stressful life experiences may be a cause of memory loss as a person ages. Glucocorticoids that are released during stress, damage neurons that are located in the hippocampal region of the brain. Therefore, the more stressful situations that someone encounters, the more susceptible they are to memory loss later on. The CA1 neurons found in the hippocampus are destroyed due to glucocorticoids decreasing the release of glucose and the reuptake of glutamate. This high level of extracellular glutamate allows calcium to enter NMDA receptors which in return kills neurons. Stressful life experiences can also cause repression of memories where a person moves an unbearable memory to the unconscious mind.[53] This directly relates to traumatic events in one's past such as kidnappings, being prisoners of war or sexual abuse as a child. The more long term the exposure to stress is, the more impact it may have. However, short term exposure to stress also causes impairment in memory by interfering with the function of the hippocampus. Research shows that subjects placed in a stressful situation for a short amount of time still have blood glucocorticoid levels that have increased drastically when measured after the exposure is completed. When subjects are asked to complete a learning task after short term exposure they often have difficulties. Prenatal stress also hinders the ability to learn and memorize by disrupting the development of the hippocampus and can lead to unestablished long term potentiation in the offspring of severely stressed parents. Although the stress is applied prenatally, the offspring show increased levels of glucocorticoids when they are subjected to stress later on in life.[85] SleepMaking memories occurs through a three-step process, which can be enhanced by sleep. The three steps are as follows:
Sleep affects memory consolidation. During sleep, the neural connections in the brain are strengthened. This enhances the brain's abilities to stabilize and retain memories. There have been several studies which show that sleep improves the retention of memory, as memories are enhanced through active consolidation. System consolidation takes place during slow-wave sleep (SWS).[86] This process implicates that memories are reactivated during sleep, but that the process doesn't enhance every memory. It also implicates that qualitative changes are made to the memories when they are transferred to long-term store during sleep. During sleep, the hippocampus replays the events of the day for the neocortex. The neocortex then reviews and processes memories, which moves them into long-term memory. When one does not get enough sleep it makes it more difficult to learn as these neural connections are not as strong, resulting in a lower retention rate of memories. Sleep deprivation makes it harder to focus, resulting in inefficient learning.[86] Furthermore, some studies have shown that sleep deprivation can lead to false memories as the memories are not properly transferred to long-term memory. One of the primary functions of sleep is thought to be the improvement of the consolidation of information, as several studies have demonstrated that memory depends on getting sufficient sleep between training and test.[87] Additionally, data obtained from neuroimaging studies have shown activation patterns in the sleeping brain that mirror those recorded during the learning of tasks from the previous day,[87] suggesting that new memories may be solidified through such rehearsal.[88] Construction for general manipulationAlthough people often think that memory operates like recording equipment, it is not the case. The molecular mechanisms underlying the induction and maintenance of memory are very dynamic and comprise distinct phases covering a time window from seconds to even a lifetime.[89] In fact, research has revealed that our memories are constructed: "current hypotheses suggest that constructive processes allow individuals to simulate and imagine future episodes, happenings, and scenarios. Since the future is not an exact repetition of the past, simulation of future episodes requires a complex system that can draw on the past in a manner that flexibly extracts and recombines elements of previous experiences – a constructive rather than a reproductive system."[61] People can construct their memories when they encode them and/or when they recall them. To illustrate, consider a classic study conducted by Elizabeth Loftus and John Palmer (1974)[90] in which people were instructed to watch a film of a traffic accident and then asked about what they saw. The researchers found that the people who were asked, "How fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?" gave higher estimates than those who were asked, "How fast were the cars going when they hit each other?" Furthermore, when asked a week later whether they had seen broken glass in the film, those who had been asked the question with smashed were twice more likely to report that they had seen broken glass than those who had been asked the question with hit. There was no broken glass depicted in the film. Thus, the wording of the questions distorted viewers' memories of the event. Importantly, the wording of the question led people to construct different memories of the event – those who were asked the question with smashed recalled a more serious car accident than they had actually seen. The findings of this experiment were replicated around the world, and researchers consistently demonstrated that when people were provided with misleading information they tended to misremember, a phenomenon known as the misinformation effect.[91] Research has revealed that asking individuals to repeatedly imagine actions that they have never performed or events that they have never experienced could result in false memories. For instance, Goff and Roediger[92] (1998) asked participants to imagine that they performed an act (e.g., break a toothpick) and then later asked them whether they had done such a thing. Findings revealed that those participants who repeatedly imagined performing such an act were more likely to think that they had actually performed that act during the first session of the experiment. Similarly, Garry and her colleagues (1996)[93] asked college students to report how certain they were that they experienced a number of events as children (e.g., broke a window with their hand) and then two weeks later asked them to imagine four of those events. The researchers found that one-fourth of the students asked to imagine the four events reported that they had actually experienced such events as children. That is, when asked to imagine the events they were more confident that they experienced the events. Research reported in 2013 revealed that it is possible to artificially stimulate prior memories and artificially implant false memories in mice. Using optogenetics, a team of RIKEN-MIT scientists caused the mice to incorrectly associate a benign environment with a prior unpleasant experience from different surroundings. Some scientists believe that the study may have implications in studying false memory formation in humans, and in treating PTSD and schizophrenia.[94][95] Improving{{Main|Improving memory}}A UCLA research study published in the June 2008 issue of the American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry found that people can improve cognitive function and brain efficiency through simple lifestyle changes such as incorporating memory exercises, healthy eating, physical fitness and stress reduction into their daily lives. This study examined 17 subjects, (average age 53) with normal memory performance. Eight subjects were asked to follow a "brain healthy" diet, relaxation, physical, and mental exercise (brain teasers and verbal memory training techniques). After 14 days, they showed greater word fluency (not memory) compared to their baseline performance. No long-term follow-up was conducted; it is therefore unclear if this intervention has lasting effects on memory.[96] There are a loosely associated group of mnemonic principles and techniques that can be used to vastly improve memory known as the art of memory. The International Longevity Center released in 2001 a report[97] which includes in pages 14–16 recommendations for keeping the mind in good functionality until advanced age. Some of the recommendations are to stay intellectually active through learning, training or reading, to keep physically active so to promote blood circulation to the brain, to socialize, to reduce stress, to keep sleep time regular, to avoid depression or emotional instability and to observe good nutrition. Memorization is a method of learning that allows an individual to recall information verbatim. Rote learning is the method most often used. Methods of memorizing things have been the subject of much discussion over the years with some writers, such as Cosmos Rossellius using visual alphabets. The spacing effect shows that an individual is more likely to remember a list of items when rehearsal is spaced over an extended period of time. In contrast to this is cramming: an intensive memorization in a short period of time. Also relevant is the Zeigarnik effect which states that people remember uncompleted or interrupted tasks better than completed ones. The so-called Method of loci uses spatial memory to memorize non-spatial information.[98]In plantsPlants lack a specialized organ devoted to memory retention, and so plant memory has been a controversial topic in recent years. New advances in the field have identified the presence of neurotransmitters in plants, adding to the hypothesis that plants are capable of remembering.[99] Action potentials, a physiological response characteristic of neurons, have been shown to have an influence on plants as well, including in wound responses and photosynthesis.[99] In addition to these homologous features of memory systems in both plants and animals, plants have also been observed to encode, store and retrieve basic short-term memories. One of the most well-studied plants to show rudimentary memory is the Venus flytrap. Native to the subtropical wetlands of the eastern United States, Venus Fly Traps have evolved the ability to obtain meat for sustenance, likely due to the lack of nitrogen in the soil.[100] This is done by two trap-forming leaf tips that snap shut once triggered by a potential prey. On each lobe, three triggers hairs await stimulation. In order to maximize the benefit to cost ratio, the plant enables a rudimentary form of memory in which two trigger hairs must be stimulated within 30 seconds in order to result in trap closure.[100] This system ensures that the trap only closes when potential prey is within grasp. The time lapse between trigger hair stimulations suggests that the plant can remember an initial stimulus long enough for a second stimulus to initiate trap closure. This memory isn't encoded in a brain, as plants lack this specialized organ. Rather, information is stored in the form of cytoplasmic calcium levels. The first trigger causes a subthreshold cytoplasmic calcium influx.[100] This initial trigger isn't enough to activate trap closure, and so a subsequent stimulus allows for a secondary influx of calcium. The latter calcium rise superimposes on the initial one, creating an action potential that passes threshold, resulting in trap closure.[100] Researchers, to prove that an electrical threshold must be met to stimulate trap closure, excited a single trigger hair with a constant mechanical stimulus using Ag/AgCl electrodes.[101] The trap closed after only a few seconds. This experiment gave evidence to demonstrate that the electrical threshold, not necessarily the number of trigger hair stimulations, was the contributing factor in Venus Fly Trap memory. It has been shown that trap closure can be blocked using uncouplers and inhibitors of voltage-gated channels.[101] After trap closure, these electrical signals stimulate glandular production of jasmonic acid and hydrolases, allowing for digestion of the prey.[102] The field of plant neurobiology has gained a large amount of interest over the past decade, leading to an influx of research regarding plant memory. Although the Venus flytrap is one of the more highly studied, many other plants exhibit the capacity to remember, including the Mimosa pudica through an experiment conducted by Monica Gagliano and colleagues in 2013.[103] As the field expands, it is likely that we will learn more about the capacity of a plant to remember. See also{{Portal|Memory}}{{col div|colwidth=30em}}
Notes1. ^{{cite book|author=Lauralee Sherwood|title=Human Physiology: From Cells to Systems|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=_i5BBAAAQBAJ&pg=PT189|date=1 January 2015|publisher=Cengage Learning|isbn=978-1-305-44551-2|pages=157–162}} 2. ^1 2 3 4 5 6 {{cite book | last=Eysenck | first=Michael | title=Attention and Arousal : Cognition and Performance | publisher=Springer Berlin Heidelberg | location=Berlin, Heidelberg | year=2012 | isbn=978-3-642-68390-9 | oclc=858929786 | page=}} 3. ^1 {{cite book | last=Baddeley | first=Alan | title=Working Memory, Thought, and Action | publisher=Oxford University Press | date=2007-03-15 | isbn=978-0-19-852801-2 | doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.001.0001 | page=}} 4. ^{{cite journal | last=Squire | first=L. R. | title=Memory and Brain Systems: 1969-2009 | journal=The Journal of Neuroscience | volume=29 | issue=41 | date=2009-10-14 | issn=0270-6474 | pmid=19828780 | pmc=2791502 | doi=10.1523/jneurosci.3575-09.2009 | pages=12711–12716}} 5. ^{{cite journal | last=Squire | first=Larry R. | title=The Legacy of Patient H.M. for Neuroscience | journal=Neuron | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=61 | issue=1 | date=2009-01-15 | issn=0896-6273 | pmid=19146808 | pmc=2649674 | doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023 | pages=6–9}} 6. ^{{cite journal | last=Graf | first=P | last2=Schacter | first2=DL | title=Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. | journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume=11 | issue=3 | year=1985 | issn=0278-7393 | pmid=3160813 | pages=501–18 | url=https://scholar.harvard.edu/files/schacterlab/files/grafschacter1985.pdf| doi=10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501 }} 7. ^{{cite journal | last=Schacter | first=Daniel L | last2=Addis | first2=Donna Rose | title=The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future | journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | publisher=The Royal Society | volume=362 | issue=1481 | date=2007-05-29 | issn=0962-8436 | pmid=17395575 | pmc=2429996 | doi=10.1098/rstb.2007.2087 | pages=773–786}} 8. ^{{cite journal | last=Schacter | first=Daniel L. | last2=Addis | first2=Donna Rose | last3=Buckner | first3=Randy L. | title=Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain | journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience | publisher=Springer Nature | volume=8 | issue=9 | year=2007 | issn=1471-003X | doi=10.1038/nrn2213 | pmid=17700624 | pages=657–661}} 9. ^{{cite journal | last=Szpunar | first=Karl K. | title=Episodic Future Thought | journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science | publisher=SAGE Publications | volume=5 | issue=2 | year=2010 | issn=1745-6916 | doi=10.1177/1745691610362350 | pmid=26162121 | pages=142–162}} 10. ^1 {{cite journal | last=Tulving | first=E | last2=Schacter | first2=DL | title=Priming and human memory systems. | journal=Science (New York, N.Y.) | volume=247 | issue=4940 | date=1990-01-19 | issn=0036-8075 | pmid=2296719 | pages=301–6| doi=10.1126/science.2296719 }} 11. ^1 2 {{cite journal | last=Foerde | first=K. | last2=Knowlton | first2=B. J. | last3=Poldrack | first3=R. A. | title=Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction | journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | publisher=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences | volume=103 | issue=31 | date=2006-07-25 | issn=0027-8424 | pmid=16868087 | pmc=1544246 | doi=10.1073/pnas.0602659103 | pages=11778–11783}} 12. ^{{cite journal | last=Squire | first=L. R. | title=Memory and Brain Systems: 1969-2009 | journal=The Journal of Neuroscience | volume=29 | issue=41 | date=2009-10-14 | issn=0270-6474 | pmid=19828780 | pmc=2791502 | doi=10.1523/jneurosci.3575-09.2009 | pages=12711–12716}} 13. ^{{cite journal | last=Squire | first=Larry R. | last2=Wixted | first2=John T. | title=The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory Since H.M. | journal=Annual Review of Neuroscience | publisher=Annual Reviews | volume=34 | issue=1 | date=2011-07-21 | issn=0147-006X | pmid=21456960 | pmc=3192650 | doi=10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720 | pages=259–288}} 14. ^{{cite journal | last=Li | first=Mi | last2=Zhong | first2=Ning | last3=Lu | first3=Shengfu | last4=Wang | first4=Gang | last5=Feng | first5=Lei | last6=Hu | first6=Bin | editor-last=Branchi | editor-first=Igor | title=Cognitive Behavioral Performance of Untreated Depressed Patients with Mild Depressive Symptoms | journal=PLoS ONE | publisher=Public Library of Science (PLoS) | volume=11 | issue=1 | date=2016-01-05 | issn=1932-6203 | pmid=26730597 | pmc=4711581 | doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0146356 | page=e0146356}} 15. ^{{cite journal | last=Bennett | first=Ilana J. | last2=Rivera | first2=Hannah G. | last3=Rypma | first3=Bart | title=Isolating age-group differences in working memory load-related neural activity: Assessing the contribution of working memory capacity using a partial-trial fMRI method | journal=NeuroImage | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=72 | date=2013-01-26 | issn=1053-8119 | pmid=23357076 | pmc=3602125 | doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.030 | pages=20–32}} 16. ^{{cite journal | last=Staniloiu | first=Angelica | last2=Markowitsch | first2=Hans J. | title=Towards Solving the Riddle of Forgetting in Functional Amnesia: Recent Advances and Current Opinions | journal=Frontiers in Psychology | publisher=Frontiers Media SA | volume=3 | pages=403 | date=2012-11-01 | issn=1664-1078 | pmid=23125838 | pmc=3485580 | doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00403 }} 17. ^{{cite journal | last=Smith | first=Christine N. | title=Retrograde memory for public events in mild cognitive impairment and its relationship to anterograde memory and neuroanatomy. | journal=Neuropsychology | publisher=American Psychological Association (APA) | volume=28 | issue=6 | date=2014-07-28 | issn=1931-1559 | pmid=25068664 | pmc=4227913 | doi=10.1037/neu0000117 | pages=959–972}} 18. ^{{cite journal | last=Ortega-de San Luis | first=Clara | last2=Ryan | first2=Tomás J. | title=United states of amnesia: rescuing memory loss from diverse conditions | journal=Disease Models & Mechanisms | publisher=The Company of Biologists | volume=11 | issue=5 | date=2018-05-01 | issn=1754-8403 | pmid=29784659 | pmc=5992608 | doi=10.1242/dmm.035055 | page=dmm035055}} 19. ^{{cite journal | last=Staniloiu | first=Angelica | last2=Markowitsch | first2=Hans J. | title=The Remains of the Day in Dissociative Amnesia | journal=Brain Sciences | publisher=MDPI AG | volume=2 | issue=2 | date=2012-04-10 | issn=2076-3425 | pmid=24962768 | pmc=4061789 | doi=10.3390/brainsci2020101 | pages=101–129}} 20. ^{{cite journal | last=Spiegel | first=David R | last2=Smith | first2=Justin | last3=Wade | first3=Ryan R | last4=Cherukuru | first4=Nithya | last5=Ursani | first5=Aneel | last6=Dobruskina | first6=Yuliya | last7=Crist | first7=Taylor | last8=Busch | first8=Robert F | last9=Dhanani | first9=Rahim M | last10=Dreyer | first10=Nicholas | title=Transient global amnesia: current perspectives | journal=Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment | publisher=Dove Medical Press Ltd. | volume=Volume 13 | date=2017-10-24 | issn=1178-2021 | pmid=29123402 | pmc=5661450 | doi=10.2147/ndt.s130710 | pages=2691–2703}} 21. ^{{cite journal | last=Bauer | first=Patricia J. | last2=Larkina | first2=Marina | title=The onset of childhood amnesia in childhood: A prospective investigation of the course and determinants of forgetting of early-life events | journal=Memory (Hove, England) | publisher=Informa UK Limited | volume=22 | issue=8 | date=2013-11-18 | issn=0965-8211 | pmid=24236647 | pmc=4025992 | doi=10.1080/09658211.2013.854806 | pages=907–924}} 22. ^{{Cite journal | author = Sperling, G| year = 1963 | title = A Model for Visual Memory Tasks | journal = Hfs.sagepub.com |url=http://hfs.sagepub.com/content/5/1/19.short#cited-by | volume = 5 | issue = 1 | pages = 19–31 }} 23. ^{{cite book |author=Carlson, Neil R. |title=Psychology: the science of behavior |publisher=Allyn & Bacon |location=Boston, Mass |year=2010 |pages= |isbn=978-0-205-68557-8 |oclc=268547522}} 24. ^{{cite journal |author=Cowan, N |title=The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity |journal=Behav Brain Sci |volume=24 |issue=1 |pages=87–114; discussion 114–85 |date=February 2001 |pmid=11515286 |url=http://langint.pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp/ai/intra_data/NobuyukiKawai/Kawai-Matsuzawa-Magical_number_5_in_a_chimpanzee.pdf |doi=10.1017/S0140525X01003922}} 25. ^{{cite journal |author=Miller, G.A. |title=The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information |journal=Psychol Rev |volume=63 |issue=2 |pages=81–97 |date=March 1956 |pmid=13310704 |doi=10.1037/h0043158|citeseerx=10.1.1.308.8071 }} 26. ^{{Cite journal | author = Conrad, R. | year = 1964 | title = Acoustic Confusions in Immediate Memory | url = http://step.psy.cmu.edu/articles/Conrad64.doc | journal = British Journal of Psychology | volume = 55 | issue = | pages = 75–84 | doi = 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1964.tb00899.x | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20070628193757/http://step.psy.cmu.edu/articles/Conrad64.doc | archivedate = 2007-06-28 | df = }} 27. ^{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1080/14640746608400047 | author = Baddeley, A.D. | year = 1966 | title = The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences | url = | journal = Q. J. Exp. Physiol. | volume = 18 | issue = 4| pages = 302–9 | pmid=5956072| title-link = semantic similarity }} 28. ^{{cite journal |author1=Clayton, N.S. |author2=Dickinson, A. |title=Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays |journal=Nature |volume=395 |issue=6699 |pages=272–4 |date=September 1998 |pmid=9751053 |doi=10.1038/26216 |url=|bibcode=1998Natur.395..272C }} 29. ^{{cite journal|year=1957|title=Loss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal Lesions|journal=Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry|volume=20|issue=1|pages=11–21|doi=10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11|pmc=497229|pmid=13406589|author1=Scoville W.B.|author2=Milner B.}} 30. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Miller C, Sweatt J |title=Covalent modification of DNA regulates memory formation |journal=Neuron |volume=53 |issue=6 |pages=857–869 |date=2007-03-15 |pmid=17359920 |doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.022}} 31. ^{{cite journal |last1=Papassotiropoulos |first1=Andreas |last2=Wollmer |first2=M. Axel |last3=Aguzzi |first3=Adriano |last4=Hock |first4=Christoph |last5=Nitsch |first5=Roger M. |last6=de Quervain |first6=Dominique J.-F. |year=2005 |title=The prion gene is associated with human long-term memory |journal=Human Molecular Genetics |volume=14 |issue=15 |pages=2241–2246 |doi=10.1093/hmg/ddi228 |url= |pmid=15987701}} 32. ^{{Cite journal|last=Buchmann|first=Andreas|last2=Mondadori|first2=Christian R. A.|last3=Hänggi|first3=Jürgen|last4=Aerni|first4=Amanda|last5=Vrticka|first5=Pascal|last6=Luechinger|first6=Roger|last7=Boesiger|first7=Peter|last8=Hock|first8=Christoph|last9=Nitsch|first9=Roger M.|date=2008|title=Prion protein M129V polymorphism affects retrieval-related brain activity|journal=Neuropsychologia|volume=46|issue=9|pages=2389–2402|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.002|issn=0028-3932|pmid=18423780}} 33. ^{{cite journal|pmid=3952419 | volume=75 | issue=7 | title=A case from practice (49). Patient: K.F., born 6 May 1930 (bird fancier's lung) | date=February 1986 |author1=Zlonoga, B. |author2=Gerber, A. | journal=Schweiz. Rundsch. Med. Prax. | pages=171–72}} 34. ^1 {{Cite journal | doi = 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 | author = Baddeley, A.D. | year = 2000 | title = The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? | url = | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 4 | issue = 11| pages = 417–23 | pmid=11058819}} 35. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.med.univ-rennes1.fr/iidris/cache/an/40/4003 |title=IIDRSI: topographic memory loss |publisher=Med.univ-rennes1.fr |date= |accessdate=2012-11-08 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20130430232457/http://www.med.univ-rennes1.fr/iidris/cache/an/40/4003 |archivedate=2013-04-30 |df= }} 36. ^{{cite journal |author1=Aguirre, G.K. |author2=D'Esposito, M. |title=Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy |journal=Brain |volume=122 |issue= 9|pages=1613–28 |date=September 1999|pmid=10468502 |doi= 10.1093/brain/122.9.1613 |url=http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/122/9/1613.long}} 37. ^T.L. Brink (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. "Unit 7: Memory." p. 120 38. ^{{cite book |author=Neisser, Ulric |title=Memory observed: remembering in natural contexts |publisher=W.H. Freeman |location=San Francisco |year=1982 |pages= |isbn=978-0-7167-1372-2 |oclc= 7837605}} 39. ^{{Cite book | last1 = Anderson | first1 = John R. | title = Language, memory, and though | year = 1976 | publisher = L. Erlbaum Associates | location = Hillsdale, NJ | isbn = 978-0-470-15187-7 | oclc = 2331424 }} 40. ^{{cite book |author1=Schacter, Daniel L |author2=Gilbert, Daniel T |author3=Wegner, Daniel M |title= Implicit Memory and Explicit Memory |work=Psychology |publisher=Worth Publishers |location=New York |year=2010 |pages=238 |isbn=978-1-4292-3719-2 |oclc=755079969 }} 41. ^{{cite journal |author=Fagan, J.F. |title=Infant recognition memory: the effects of length of familiarization and type of discrimination task |journal=Child Dev |volume=45 |issue=2 |pages=351–356 |date=June 1974 |pmid=4837713 |jstor=1127955 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8624.1974.tb00603.x }} 42. ^{{cite journal|last1=Rovee-Collier|first1=Carolyn|title=The Development of Infant Memory|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|volume=8| issue=3|year=1999| pages=80–85| issn=0963-7214|doi=10.1111/1467-8721.00019 |url=http://bernard.pitzer.edu/~dmoore/psych199s03articles/R-Collier_memory.pdf}} 43. ^{{cite book |author=Rovee-Collier, C.K., Bhatt, R.S. | title=Evidence of long-term retention in infancy |work=Annals of Child Development |editor= Ross Vasta |publisher=Jessica Kingsley Pub |location=London |volume=9 |year=1993 |pages=1–45 |isbn=978-1-85302-219-7 |oclc=827689578}} 44. ^{{cite journal |author1=Hartshorn, K. |author2=Rovee-Collier, C. |author3=Gerhardstein, P. |title=The ontogeny of long-term memory over the first year-and-a-half of life |journal=Dev Psychobiol |volume=32 |issue=2 |pages=69–89 |date=March 1998 |pmid=9526683 |doi=10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199803)32:2<69::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-Q |url=|display-authors=etal}} 45. ^1 {{cite journal |author=Meltzoff, A.N. |title=What infant memory tells us about infantile amnesia: long-term recall and deferred imitation |journal=J Exp Child Psychol |volume=59 |issue=3 |pages=497–515 |date=June 1995 |pmid=7622990 |pmc=3629912 |doi=10.1006/jecp.1995.1023 |url=}} 46. ^1 {{cite journal|last1=Bauer|first1=Patricia J.| title=Long-Term Recall Memory: Behavioral and Neuro-Developmental Changes in the First 2 Years of Life|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|volume=11| issue=4|year=2002| pages=137–141|issn=0963-7214|doi=10.1111/1467-8721.00186}} 47. ^{{cite book |author=Bauer, Patricia J. |title=Remembering the times of our lives: memory in infancy and beyond |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Hillsdale, NJ |year=2007 |pages= |isbn=978-0-8058-5733-7 |oclc=62089961 }} 48. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/438701/paired-associate-learning |title=Paired-associate learning |work=Encyclopædia Britannica }} 49. ^{{cite journal |author=Kesner RP |title=A process analysis of the CA3 subregion of the hippocampus |journal=Front Cell Neurosci |volume=7 |issue= |pages=78 |year=2013 |pmid=23750126 |pmc=3664330 |doi=10.3389/fncel.2013.00078 }} 50. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/493353/recall |title=Recall (memory) |work=Encyclopædia Britannica }} 51. ^Baddeley, Alan D., "The Psychology of Memory", pp. 131–132, Basic Books, Inc., Publishers, New York, 1976, {{ISBN|0465067360}} 52. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/493614/recognition |title=Recognition (memory) |work=Encyclopædia Britannica }} 53. ^1 2 3 {{Cite book|title = Introduction to Psychology|last = Kalat|first = James W.|publisher = Wadsworth Cengage Learning|isbn = 978-1-133-95660-0|location = Canada|pages = |year= 2013}} 54. ^{{cite journal |author1=LaBar K.S. |author2=Cabeza R. |title=Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=7 |pages=54–64 |year=2006 |doi=10.1038/nrn1825 |pmid=16371950 |issue=1}} 55. ^{{Cite book|title=Memory and the computational brain: Why cognitive science will transform neuroscience|author1=Gallistel C.R. |author2=King A.P. |publisher=Wiley-Blackwell|year=2009|isbn=978-1-4051-2288-7|location=Chichester, England|pages=|quote=}} 56. ^{{Cite journal|last=Gallistel|first=C.R.|last2=Matzel|first2=Louis D.|date=2013-01-02|title=The Neuroscience of Learning: Beyond the Hebbian Synapse|journal=Annual Review of Psychology|volume=64|issue=1|pages=169–200|doi=10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143807|pmid=22804775|issn=0066-4308|doi-access=}} 57. ^{{Cite journal|last=Trettenbrein|first=P.C.|date=2016-01-01|title=The Demise of the Synapse As the Locus of Memory: A Looming Paradigm Shift?|journal=Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience|volume=10 | issue = 88 |pages=88|doi=10.3389/fnsys.2016.00088|pmid=27909400|pmc=5112247|doi-access=free}} 58. ^{{cite journal |author1=Adolphs R. |author2=Cahill L. |author3=Schul R. |author4=Babinsky R. |title=Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans |journal=Learning & Memory |volume=4 |issue=3 |pages=291–300 |year=1997 |doi=10.1101/lm.4.3.291}} 59. ^{{cite journal |author1=Cahill L.|author2=Babinsky R.|author3=Markowitsch H.J.|author4=McGaugh J.L.|authorlink3=Hans Markowitsch| title=The amygdala and emotional memory |journal=Nature |volume=377 |pages=295–296 |year=1995 |doi=10.1038/377295a0 |pmid=7566084|issue=6547|bibcode=1995Natur.377..295C}} 60. ^{{cite book| last =Kalat| first =J.W.| title =Biological psychology (7th ed.)| publisher = Wadsworth Publishing| year =2001 | location =Belmont, CA }} 61. ^1 {{cite journal | author = Ofengenden Tzofit | year = 2014 | title = Memory formation and belief | url = http://www.crossingdialogues.com/Ms-A14-03.pdf | journal = Dialogues in Philosophy, Mental and Neuro Sciences | volume = 7 | issue = 2| pages = 34–44 }} 62. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.news-medical.net/news/2006/10/20/20650.aspx |title=Gene called Kibra plays an important role in memory |publisher=News-medical.net |date= 2006-10-20|accessdate=2012-11-08}} 63. ^{{cite web | url=https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/05/180514151920.htm | title=Biologists 'transfer' a memory through RNA injection: Research in marine snails could lead to new treatments to restore memories and alter traumatic ones}} 64. ^{{cite book |author=Teti D.M. |title=Handbook of research methods in developmental science: New developments in the study of infant memory |publisher=Blackwell Publishing |location=San Francisco |year=2005}} 65. ^1 2 {{cite journal |author1=Barr R. |author2=Dowden A. |author3=Hayne H. |title=Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6- to 24-month-old infants |journal=Infant Behavior and Development |volume=19 |issue=2 |pages=159–170 |year=1996 |doi=10.1016/s0163-6383(96)90015-6}} 66. ^{{cite journal |author=Bauer P.J. |title=Getting explicit memory off the ground: Steps toward construction of a neuro-developmental account of changes in the first two years of life |journal=Developmental Review |volume=24 |issue=4 |pages=347–373 |year=2004 |doi=10.1016/j.dr.2004.08.003}} 67. ^{{cite journal |author1=Bauer, P.J. |author2=Wiebe, S.A. |author3=Carver, L.J. |author4=Waters, J.M. |author5=Nelson, C.A. |title=Developments in long-term explicit memory late in the first year of life: behavioral and electrophysiological indices |journal=Psychol Sci |volume=14 |issue=6 |pages=629–35 |date=November 2003 |pmid=14629697 |doi=10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1476.x }} 68. ^{{cite journal |author1=Carver, L.J. |author2=Bauer, P.J. |title=When the event is more than the sum of its parts: 9-month-olds' long-term ordered recall |journal=Memory |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=147–174 |date=March 1999 |pmid=10645377 |doi=10.1080/741944070 }} 69. ^{{cite journal |author1=Carver, L.J. |author2=Bauer, P.J. |title=The dawning of a past: the emergence of long-term explicit memory in infancy |journal=J Exp Psychol Gen |volume=130 |issue=4 |pages=726–45 |date=December 2001 |pmid=11757877 |doi=10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.726 |citeseerx=10.1.1.130.4890 }} 70. ^{{cite book |editor=Cowan, N. |title=The development of memory in childhood |publisher=Psychology Press |location=Hove, East Sussex |year=1997}} 71. ^{{Cite journal|title = Ontogeny of memory: An update on 40 years of work on infantile amnesia|journal = Behavioural Brain Research|date = 2016-02-01|pages = 4–14|volume = 298|issue= Part A|series = Developmental Regulation of Memory in Anxiety and Addiction|doi = 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.030|first = Heather Bronwyn|last = Madsen|first2 = Jee Hyun|last2 = Kim|pmid=26190765}} 72. ^{{cite journal |author1=Parkin A.J. |author2=Walter B.M. |author3=Hunkin N.M. |title=Relationships between normal aging, frontal lobe function, and memory for temporal and spatial information |journal=Neuropsychology |volume=9 |issue=3 |pages=304–312 |year=1995 |doi=10.1037/0894-4105.9.3.304}} 73. ^{{cite journal |author1=McIntyer J.S. |author2=Craik F.I.M. |title=Age differences in memory for item and source information |journal=Canadian Journal of Psychology |volume=41 |issue=2 |pages=175–192 |year=1987 |doi=10.1037/h0084154 |pmid=3502895}} 74. ^1 {{cite journal |vauthors=Lu T, Pan Y, Kao SY, Li C, Kohane I, Chan J, Yankner BA |title=Gene regulation and DNA damage in the ageing human brain |journal=Nature |volume=429 |issue=6994 |pages=883–91 |date=June 2004 |pmid=15190254 |doi=10.1038/nature02661 |url=}} 75. ^{{cite journal |author=Corkin S, Amaral DG, Gonzalez RG, Johnson KA, Hyman, BT |title=H.M.'s medial temporal lobe lesion: Findings from magnetic resonance imaging |journal=The Journal of Neuroscience |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=3964–3979 |year=1997|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03964.1997 }} 76. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Zola-Morgan S, Suire LR |title=Neuroanatomy of memory |journal=Annual Review of Neuroscience |volume=16 |pages=547–563 |year=1993 |doi=10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002555 |pmid=8460903}} 77. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.columbia.edu/cu/record/archives/vol21/vol21_iss22/record2122.15.html |title=Memory of Time May Be Factor in Parkinson's |publisher=Columbia.edu |date=1996-04-05 |accessdate=2012-11-08}} 78. ^[https://www.newscientist.com/channel/being-human/mg19726431.600-forgetfulness-is-key-to-a-healthy-mind.html Forgetfulness is the Key to a Healthy Mind]. New Scientist, February 16. 2008. 79. ^{{cite journal |author=Underwood BJ |title=Interference and forgetting |journal=Psychological Review |volume=64 |issue=1 |pages=49–60 |year=1957 |doi=10.1037/h0044616|pmid=13408394 }} 80. ^{{cite book |vauthors=Perkins DN, Salomon G |title=Transfer of learning |editor1=Postlethwaite, T. Neville|editor2=Husén, Torsten |work=International Encyclopedia of Education |edition=2 |publisher=Pergamon |location=Oxford |year=1992 |isbn=978-0-08-041046-3 |oclc=749308019 |url=http://learnweb.harvard.edu/alps/thinking/docs/traencyn.htm }} 81. ^{{cite journal | author = Conrad C.D. | year = 2010 | title = A critical review of chronic stress effects on spatial learning and memory | url = | journal = Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry | volume = 34 | issue = 5| pages = 742–755 | doi = 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.11.003 | pmid = 19903505 }} 82. ^{{cite journal|last1=Schwabe|first1=L.|last2=Wolf|first2=O.T.|title=Learning under stress impairs memory formation|journal=Neurobiology of Learning and Memory|year=2010|volume=93|issue=2|pages=183–188|doi=10.1016/j.nlm.2009.09.009|pmid=19796703}} 83. ^{{cite journal|last1=Schwabe|first1=L.|last2=Wolf|first2=O.T.|title=The context counts: Congruent learning and testing environments prevent memory retrieval impairment following stress|journal=Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience|year=2009|volume=9|issue=3|pages=229–236|doi=10.3758/CABN.9.3.229|pmid=19679758}} 84. ^{{cite journal|last1=Schwabe|first1=L.|last2=Bohringer|first2=A.|last3=Wolf|first3=O.T.|title=Stress disrupts context-dependent memory|journal=Learning and Memory|year=2009|volume=16|issue=2|pages=110–113|doi=10.1101/lm.1257509|pmid=19181616}} 85. ^{{cite book|last=Carlson|first=N.|year=2013|title=Physiology of Behavior, eleventh edition|location=Upper Saddle River, NJ |publisher=Pearson}} 86. ^1 {{cite web|last1=Karriem-Norwood|first1=Varnada|title=Sleep Deprivation and Memory Loss|url=http://www.webmd.com/sleep-disorders/sleep-deprivation-effects-on-memory|website=Web MD|publisher=Web MD LLC|accessdate=November 20, 2014}} 87. ^1 {{cite journal|author1=Ellenbogen, J.M. |author2=Hulbert, J.C. |author3=Stickgold, R. |author4=Dinges, D.F. |author5=Thompson-Schill, S.L. |title=Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference |journal=Curr. Biol. |volume=16 |issue=13 |pages=1290–1294 |date=July 2006 |pmid=16824917 |doi=10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.024 |url=http://www.med.upenn.edu/uep/user_documents/Ellenbogen2006-Interferingwiththeoriesofsleepandmemory-sleepdeclarativememoryandassociativei.pdf |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20140514201825/http://www.med.upenn.edu/uep/user_documents/Ellenbogen2006-Interferingwiththeoriesofsleepandmemory-sleepdeclarativememoryandassociativei.pdf |archivedate=2014-05-14 |df= }} 88. ^{{cite journal|last1=Alhola|first1=Paula|title=Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance|pmc=2656292|pmid=19300585|volume=3|issue=5|year=2007|journal=Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat|pages=553–67}} 89. ^Schwarzel. M.& Mulluer. U., "Dynamic Memory Networks", "Cellular and Molecular Life Science", 2006 90. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Loftus EF, Palmer JC|title=Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory |journal=Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior |volume=13 |issue=5 |pages=585–589 |year=1974 |doi=10.1016/s0022-5371(74)80011-3}} 91. ^{{cite journal |author=Loftus GR |title=When a lie becomes memory's truth: Memory distortion after exposure to misinformation |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=1 |issue=4 |pages=121–123 |year=1992 |doi=10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769035}} 92. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Goff LM, Roediger HL|title=Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections |journal=Memory and Cognition |volume=26 |pages=20–33 |year=1998 |doi=10.3758/bf03211367}} 93. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Garry M, Manning CG, Loftus EF, Sherman SJ |title=Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred |journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=208–214 |year=1996 |doi=10.3758/bf03212420|pmid=24213869 }} 94. ^{{cite news |title=Scientists can implant false memories into mice |first=Melissa |last=Hogenboom |url=https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/science-environment-23447600 |newspaper=BBC News |date=July 25, 2013 |access-date=July 26, 2013}} 95. ^"A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory." (video) — the scientists' June 2013 TED talk. 96. ^{{cite journal |author1=Small, G.W. |author2=Silverman, D.H. |author3=Siddarth, P. |title=Effects of a 14-day healthy longevity lifestyle program on cognition and brain function |journal=Am J Geriatr Psychiatry |volume=14 |issue=6 |pages=538–45 |date=June 2006 |pmid=16731723 |doi=10.1097/01.JGP.0000219279.72210.ca |url=|display-authors=etal}} 97. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.ilcusa.org/_lib/pdf/isoa.pdf |title=International Longevity Center report on memory |accessdate=1 September 2008 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20070719053600/http://www.ilcusa.org/_lib/pdf/ISOA.pdf |archivedate=19 July 2007 |df= }} 98. ^{{cite journal | author = Olsson Henrik, Poom Leo, Treisman Anne | year = 2005 | title = Visual memory needs categories | url = | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 102 | issue = 24| pages = 8776–8780 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0500810102 | pmid = 15937119 | pmc = 1150822 | bibcode = 2005PNAS..102.8776O }} 99. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Baluška|first=František|last2=Mancuso|first2=Stefano|date=2009-02-01|title=Plant neurobiology: from sensory biology, via plant communication, to social plant behavior|journal=Cognitive Processing|language=en|volume=10|issue=1|pages=3–7|doi=10.1007/s10339-008-0239-6|pmid=18998182|issn=1612-4782}} 100. ^1 2 3 {{Cite journal|last=Hedrich|first=Rainer|last2=Neher|first2=Erwin|year=2018|title=Venus Flytrap: How an Excitable, Carnivorous Plant Works|journal=Trends in Plant Science|volume=23|issue=3|pages=220–234|doi=10.1016/j.tplants.2017.12.004|pmid=29336976|issn=1360-1385}} 101. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Volkov|first=Alexander G.|last2=Carrell|first2=Holly|last3=Baldwin|first3=Andrew|last4=Markin|first4=Vladislav S.|date=2009|title=Electrical memory in Venus flytrap|journal=Bioelectrochemistry|volume=75|issue=2|pages=142–147|doi=10.1016/j.bioelechem.2009.03.005|pmid=19356999|issn=1567-5394}} 102. ^{{Cite journal|last=Stokstad|first=Erik|date=2016-05-13|title=How the Venus flytrap acquired its taste for meat|journal=Science|language=en|volume=352|issue=6287|pages=756|doi=10.1126/science.352.6287.756|issn=0036-8075|pmid=27174967}} 103. ^{{Cite journal|last=Gagliano|first=Monica|last2=Renton|first2=Michael|last3=Depczynski|first3=Martial|last4=Mancuso|first4=Stefano|date=2014-05-01|title=Experience teaches plants to learn faster and forget slower in environments where it matters|journal=Oecologia|language=en|volume=175|issue=1|pages=63–72|doi=10.1007/s00442-013-2873-7|pmid=24390479|issn=0029-8549|bibcode=2014Oecol.175...63G}} References{{Refbegin|30em}}
Further reading
External links{{Library resources box|by=no|onlinebooks=no|about=yes|wikititle=memory}}{{Americana Poster|Memory and its Disorders|Memory}}{{Sister project links}}
|title = Articles related to memory |list ={{Human intelligence topics}}{{Memory}}{{Nervous system physiology}}{{Mental processes}} }}{{Authority control}} 4 : Memory|Neuropsychological assessment|Sources of knowledge|Mental processes |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。