词条 | Waggle dance |
释义 |
The waggle dance and the round dance are two forms of dance behaviour that are part of a continuous transition. As the distance between the resource and the hive increases, the round dance transforms into variations of a transitional dance, which, when communicating resources at even greater distances, becomes the waggle dance.[3] In the case of Apis mellifera ligustica, the round dance is performed until the resource is about 10 metres away from the hive, transitional dances are performed when the resource is at a distance of 20 to 30 metres away from the hive, and finally, when it is located at distances greater than 40 metres from the hive, the waggle dance is performed.[3] However, even close to the nest, the round dance can contain elements of the waggle dance, such as a waggle portion.[4] It has therefore been suggested that the term "waggle dance" is better for describing both the waggle dance and the round dance.[5] Austrian ethologist and Nobel laureate Karl von Frisch was one of the first who translated the meaning of the waggle dance.[6] DescriptionA waggle dance consists of one to 100 or more circuits, each of which consists of two phases: the waggle phase and the return phase. A worker bee's waggle dance involves running through a small figure-eight pattern: a waggle run (aka waggle phase) followed by a turn to the right to circle back to the starting point (aka return phase), another waggle run, followed by a turn and circle to the left, and so on in a regular alternation between right and left turns after waggle runs. Waggle-dancing bees produce and release two alkanes, tricosane and pentacosane, and two alkenes, (Z)-9-tricosene and (Z)-9-pentacosene, onto their abdomens and into the air.[7] The direction and duration of waggle runs are closely correlated with the direction and distance of the resource being advertised by the dancing bee. In an experiment with capture and relocation of bees exposed to a waggle dance the bees followed the path that would have taken them to an experimental feeder had they not been displaced.[1] The resource can include the location of a food source or a potential nesting site.[8] For cavity-nesting honey bees, like the western honey bee (Apis mellifera) or Apis nigrocincta, flowers that are located directly in line with the sun are represented by waggle runs in an upward direction on the vertical combs, and any angle to the right or left of the sun is coded by a corresponding angle to the right or left of the upward direction. The distance between hive and recruitment target is encoded in the duration of the waggle runs.[1][9] The farther the target, the longer the waggle phase. The more excited the bee is about the location, the more rapidly it will waggle, so it will grab the attention of the observing bees, and try to convince them. If multiple bees are doing the waggle dance, it's a competition to convince the observing bees to follow their lead, and competing bees may even disrupt other bees' dances or fight each other off. In addition, some open-air nesting honeybees such as the black dwarf honeybee (Apis andreniformis), whose nests hang from twigs or branches, will perform a horizontal dance on a stage above their nest in order to signal to resources.[10] Waggle dancing bees that have been in the nest for an extended time adjust the angles of their dances to accommodate the changing direction of the sun. Therefore, bees that follow the waggle run of the dance are still correctly led to the food source even though its angle relative to the sun has changed. The consumption of ethanol by foraging bees has been shown to reduce waggle dance activity and increase occurrence of the tremble dance.[11] Kevin Abbott and Reuven Dukas of McMaster University in Hamilton, Ontario, Canada discovered that if a dead western honeybee is placed on a flower, bees performed far fewer waggle dances upon returning to the hive. The scientists explain that the bees associate the dead bee with the presence of a predator at the food source. The reduction of the dance repetition frequency, therefore, indicates that the dancing bees perform and communicate a form of risk/benefit analysis.[12][13] Though first decoded by Karl von Frisch, dancing behavior in bees had been observed and described multiple times prior. Around 100 years before Frisch's discovery, Nicholas Unhoch described dancing behavior of bees as being an indulgence "in certain pleasures and jollity".[6] He admitted ignorance as to the purpose of the dancing. 35 years prior to Unhoch's observations, Ernst Spitzner observed bees dancing and interpreted it as transmitting forage resource odors to other nestmates.[6] Aristotle, in addition to describing flower constancy behavior, suspected that some form of communication occurred between foragers within a nest:
Jürgen Tautz also writes about it in his book The Buzz about Bees (2008):
MechanismHoneybees accumulate an electric charge during flying and when their body parts are moved or rubbed together. Bees emit constant and modulated electric fields during the waggle dance. Both low- and high-frequency components emitted by dancing bees induce passive antennal movements in stationary bees according to Coulomb's Law. The electrically charged flagellum of mechanoreceptor cells are moved by electric fields and more strongly so if sound and electric fields interact. Recordings from axons of the Johnston's organ indicate its sensitivity to electric fields. Therefore, it has been suggested that electric fields emanating from the surface charge of bees stimulate mechanoreceptors and may play a role in social communication during the waggle dance.[16]ControversyThe dance language vs. the waggle danceAs defined by von Frisch, Tanzsprache (German for "dance language") is the information about direction, distance, and quality of a resource (such as food or nesting sites) contained within the waggle dance.[17] There is supporting evidence of the waggle dance and "Tanzsprache" in Apis dorsata. Similar to other bees, they utilize the dance language to indicate the critical information regarding food resources. The dancer's body points in the direction of the food source and the sound produced during the dance indicates the profitability of the food.[18] Although there is some evidence for a direct connection between the Tanzsprache and the performance of the waggle dance, recent criticism holds that potential foragers need not correctly translate the dance language from the waggle dance to successfully forage.[17] In an experiment on the honeybee Apis mellifera, most individuals who thoroughly followed a waggle dance ignored the resource direction and location information. Instead, 93% of the foragers returned to foraging areas they had previous knowledge of.[17] Bees that follow a waggle dance can successfully forage without decoding the dance language information in several ways:[19]
Dance language as a languageThe use of the word "language" may lead to misrepresentations of the waggle dance. The Swiss linguist Ferdinand de Saussure proposed a system of language where a sign is made up two chief components. The signifier is the physical or phonetic representation of a sign. The signified is the conceptual component.[20] If the dance language followed the Saussurian dyadic model of semiotics, the signifier would be the waggle dance and the signified would be the location of the foraging resource. Though the dance language may or may not follow this sort of pattern, it is not considered to be a language with syntactical grammar or a set of symbols.[19] Efficiency and adaptationThe waggle dance may be less efficient than once thought. Some bees observe over 50 waggle runs without successfully foraging, while others will forage successfully after observing 5 runs.[19] Likewise, studies have found that honeybees rarely make use of the information communicated in the waggle dance and seem to only do so about ten percent of the time.[30][21] Evidently there is a conflict between private information, or individual experience, and social information transmitted through dance communication. This sheds light on the fact that following social information is more energetically costly than foraging independently and is not always advantageous.[22][23] Using olfactory cues and memory of plentiful foraging sites, honeybees are able to successfully forage independently without expending the potentially extensive energy it takes to process and execute the directions communicated by their fellow foragers. The waggle dance may be adaptive in some environments and not in others, which provides a plausible explanation as to why the information provided by waggle dances are only used sparingly. Depending on weather, other competitors, and food source characteristics, transmitted information may become obsolete quickly.[24] As a result, foragers reported to be attached to their food sites and continue to revisit a single patch many times after it has become unprofitable.[25] For example, the waggle dance plays a significantly larger role in foraging when food sources are not as abundant.[26][27] In temperate habitats, for instance, honey bee colonies routinely perform the waggle dance, but can still successfully forage when the dance is experimentally obscured. In tropical habitats, honey bee foraging is severely impaired if waggle dancing is prevented. This is thought to be due to the patchiness of resources in tropical environment versus the homogeneity of resources in temperate environments. In the tropics, food resources can come in the form of flowering trees which are rich in nectar but are scattered sparsely and bloom only briefly. Thus, in tropical zones information about forage location might be more valuable than in temperate zones.[28] EvolutionAncestors to modern honeybees most likely performed excitatory movements to encourage other nestmates to forage. These excitatory movements include shaking, zig-zagging, buzzing and crashing into nestmates. Similar behavior is observed in other Hymenoptera including stingless bees, wasps, bumblebees and ants.[19] The waggle dance is thought to have evolved to aid in communicating information about a new nest site, rather than spatial information about foraging sites.[19] Observations have suggested that different species of honeybees have different "dialects" of the waggle dance, each species or subspecies dance varying by curve or duration.[29][30] A study from 2008 demonstrated that a mixed colony of Asiatic honeybees (Apis cerana cerana) and European honeybees (Apis mellifera ligustica) were gradually able to understand one another's "dialects" of waggle dance.[31] Applications to operations researchIn line with recent work in swarm intelligence research involving optimization algorithms inspired by the behavior of social insects (including bees, ants and termites), and vertebrates such as fish and birds, there has recently been research on using bee waggle dance behavior for efficient fault-tolerant routing.[32] From the abstract of Wedde, Farooq, and Zhang (2004):[33] In this paper we present a novel routing algorithm, BeeHive, which has been inspired by the communicative and evaluative methods and procedures of honey bees. In this algorithm, bee agents travel through network regions called foraging zones. On their way their information on the network state is delivered for updating the local routing tables. BeeHive is fault tolerant, scalable, and relies completely on local, or regional, information, respectively. We demonstrate through extensive simulations that BeeHive achieves a similar or better performance compared to state-of-the-art algorithms. Another bee-inspired stigmergic computational technique called bee colony optimization is employed in Internet Server Optimization.[34][35] The Zigbee RF protocol is named after the waggle dance. See also
References1. ^1 2 {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1038/nature03526| pmid = 15889092| year = 2005| last1 = Riley | first1 = J.| last2 = Greggers | first2 = U.| last3 = Smith | first3 = A.| last4 = Reynolds | first4 = D.| last5 = Menzel | first5 = R.| title = The flight paths of honeybees recruited by the waggle dance| volume = 435| issue = 7039| pages = 205–207| journal = Nature |bibcode = 2005Natur.435..205R }} 2. ^{{cite journal |author1=Seeley T.D. |author2=Visscher P.K. |author3=Passino K.M. | year = 2006 | title = Group decision making in honey bee swarms | url = | journal = American Scientist | volume = 94 | issue = 3| pages = 220–229 | doi=10.1511/2006.3.220}} 3. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Rinderer|first=T. E.|last2=Beaman|first2=L. D.|title=Genic control of honey bee dance language dialect|journal=Theoretical and Applied Genetics|language=en|volume=91|issue=5|pages=727–732|doi=10.1007/bf00220950|pmid=24169907|issn=0040-5752|date=October 1995}} 4. ^{{Cite journal|last=Grüter|first=Christoph|last2=Farina|first2=Walter M.|title=The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps?|journal=Trends in Ecology & Evolution|volume=24|issue=5|pages=242–247|doi=10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.007|pmid=19307042|date=May 2009}} 5. ^{{Cite journal|last=Gardner|first=Kathryn E.|last2=Seeley|first2=Thomas D.|last3=Calderone|first3=Nicholas W.|date=2008-04-01|title=Do honeybees have two discrete dances to advertise food sources?|journal=Animal Behaviour|volume=75|issue=4|pages=1291–1300|doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.032}} 6. ^1 2 Frisch, Karl von. (1967) The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Cambridge, Mass.: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. 7. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050228| pmid = 17713987| year = 2007| last1 = Thom | first1 = C.| last2 = Gilley | first2 = D.| last3 = Hooper | first3 = J.| last4 = Esch | first4 = H.| title = The scent of the waggle dance| volume = 5| issue = 9| pages = e228| pmc = 1994260| journal = PLoS Biology }} 8. ^{{Cite web|title = The Waggle Dance|url = https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/nature/waggle-dance.html|website = www.pbs.org|access-date = 2016-01-27}} 9. ^Radloff, Sara E.; Hepburn, H. Randall; Engel, Michael S. (2011). Honeybees of Asia. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media. {{ISBN|978-3642164217}}. 10. ^{{Cite journal|title = Phylogenetic analysis of honey bee behavioral evolution|journal = Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution|date = 2007-05-01|pages = 543–552|volume = 43|issue = 2|doi = 10.1016/j.ympev.2006.10.013|pmid = 17123837|first = Rika|last = Raffiudin|first2 = Ross H.|last2 = Crozier}} 11. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.01.005| pmid = 16905444| year = 2006| last1 = Bozic | first1 = J.| last2 = Abramson | first2 = C.| last3 = Bedencic | first3 = M.| title = Reduced ability of ethanol drinkers for social communication in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Poll.)| volume = 38| issue = 3| pages = 179–183| journal = Alcohol (Fayetteville, N.Y.) }} 12. ^{{Cite news | last1 = Walker | first1 = Matt | title = Honeybees warn of Risky Flowers | newspaper = BBC Earth News | date = 31 July 2009 | url = http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8176000/8176878.stm | accessdate = 18 January 2010 }} 13. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.029| title = Honeybees consider flower danger in their waggle dance| year = 2009| last1 = Abbott | first1 = K. R.| last2 = Dukas | first2 = R.| journal = Animal Behaviour| volume = 78| issue = 3| pages = 633–635 }} 14. ^Aristotle, Historia animalium, IX, 40, Becker 624b; modified from the translation by D.W. Thompson in The Works of Aristotle, Clarendon, Oxford, 1910. 15. ^Tautz, J.: The Buzz about Bees - Biology of a Superorganism (photos by H. R. Heilmann) Springer Heidelberg & Berlin, 2008. Page 112. 16. ^{{cite journal|last=Greggers|first=Uwe |author2=Koch G |author3=Schmidt V |author4=Dürr A |author5=Floriou-Servou A |author6=Piepenbrock D |author7=Göpfert MC |author8=Menzel R|title=Reception and learning of electric fields|journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B|date=March 2013|doi=10.1098/rspb.2013.0528|volume=280|issue=1759 |pages=20130528 |pmid=23536603 |pmc=3619523}} 17. ^1 2 {{Cite journal | doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0186 | pmid=18331980 | year=2008 | last1=Grüter | first1=C. | last2=Balbuena | first2=M. | last3=Farina | first3=W. | title=Informational conflicts created by the waggle dance | volume=275 | issue=1640 | pages=1321–1327 | pmc=2602683 | journal=Proceedings: Biological Sciences}} 18. ^{{cite journal |last1=Kirchner |first1=Wolfgang |title=Acoustic signals in the dance language of the giant honeybee, Apis dorsata |journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology |issue=33 |pages=67–72}} 19. ^1 2 3 4 {{Cite journal | doi=10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.007 | pmid=19307042 | year=2009 | last1=Grüter | first1=C. | last2=Farina | first2=W. | title=The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? | volume=24 | issue=5 | pages=242–247 | journal=Trends in Ecology & Evolution}} 20. ^Saussure, Ferdinand de (1916), "Nature of the Linguistics Sign", in: Charles Bally & Albert Sechehaye (Ed.), Cours de linguistique générale, McGraw Hill Education. 21. ^{{cite journal |last1=Gruter |first1=C. |last2=Balbuena |first2=M. S. |last3=Farina |first3=W. M. |title=. Informational conflicts created by the waggle dance |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |date=2008 |volume=275 |issue=1640 |pages=1321–1327 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2008.0186 |pmid=18331980 |pmc=2602683}} 22. ^{{cite journal |last1=Al Toufailia |first1=H. |last2=Gruter |first2=C. |last3=Ratnieks |first3=F. L. W. |title=Persistence to unrewarding feeding locations by honeybee foragers (Apis mellifera): the effects of experience, resource profitability and season |journal=Ethology |date=2013 |volume=119 |issue=12 |pages=1096–1106 |doi=10.1111/eth.12170}} 23. ^{{cite journal |last1=Gruter |first1=C |last2=Ratnieks |first2=F. L. W. |title=Honeybee foragers increase the use of waggle dance information when private information becomes unrewarding |journal=Animal Behaviour |date=2011 |volume=81 |issue=5 |pages=949–954 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.014 }} 24. ^{{cite journal |last1=Gruter |first1=C. |last2=Farina |first2=W. M. |title=The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? |journal=Trends in Ecology & Evolution |date=2009 |volume=24 |issue=5 |pages=242–247 |doi=10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.007 |pmid=19307042}} 25. ^name=Persistence to unrewarding feeding locations by honeybee foragers (Apis mellifera): the effects of experience, resource profitability and season" 26. ^name="The biology of the dance language" 27. ^name="Informational conflicts created by the waggle dance" 28. ^1 {{Cite journal | doi=10.1007/s00265-003-0726-9 | title=Why do honey bees dance? | year=2004 | last1=Dornhaus | first1=A. | last2=Chittka | first2=L. | journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | volume=55 | issue=4 | pages=395–401}} 29. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1086/284972| journal = American Naturalist| year = 1989| volume = 134| issue = 1| title = On the Evolution of the Dance Language: Response to Dyer and Seeley| author = Gould| author2 = Towne| jstor = 2462282| pages = 156–159}} 30. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1007/BF00175094| journal = Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology| year = 1991| volume = 28| issue = 4| title = Dance Dialects and Foraging Range in Three Asian Honey Bee Species| author = Dyer| author2 = Seeley| jstor = 4600541| pages = 227–233}} 31. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0002365| pmid = 18523550| year = 2008| last1 = Su | first1 = S.| last2 = Cai | first2 = F.| last3 = Si | first3 = A.| last4 = Zhang | first4 = S.| last5 = Tautz | first5 = J.| last6 = Chen | first6 = S.| title = East learns from West: Asiatic honeybees can understand dance language of European honeybees| volume = 3| editor1-first = Martin| issue = 6| pages = e2365| editor1-last = Giurfa| pmc = 2391287| journal = PLoS ONE| last7 = Giurfa | first7 = M. |bibcode = 2008PLoSO...3.2365S }} 32. ^Crina, Grosan; Abraham Ajith. (2006) Stigmergic Optimization: Inspiration, Technologies and Perspectives. Studies in Computational Intelligence. Vol. 31. pp. 1-24. Springer Berlin / Heidelberg. {{ISBN|978-3-540-34689-0}} 33. ^{{Cite book| doi = 10.1145/1068009.1068034| title = Proceedings of the 2005 conference on Genetic and evolutionary computation - GECCO '05| isbn = 978-1595930101| year = 2005| last1 = Wedde | first1 = H. F.| last2 = Farooq | first2 = M.| last3 = Pannenbaecker | first3 = T.| chapter = BeeAdHoc| last4 = Vogel | first4 = B.| last5 = Mueller | first5 = C.| last6 = Meth | first6 = J.| last7 = Jeruschkat | first7 = R.| pages = 153 }} 34. ^{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1177/105971230401200308| title = On Honey Bees and Dynamic Server Allocation in Internet Hosting Centers | first2 = C.| year = 2004| last2 = Tovey| last1 = Nakrani | first1 = S.| journal = Adaptive Behavior| volume = 12| issue = 3–4| pages = 223–240 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.115.3457 }} 35. ^C. Tovey, (2004) "The Honey Bee Algorithm: A Biological Inspired Approach to Internet Server Optimization". Engineering Enterprise. Spring 2004, pp.13-15. Further reading
External links{{Commons category|Waggle dance}}
3 : Animal communication|Western honey bee behavior|Neuroethology |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。