请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Eyes-on-the-Road-Benefit
释义

  1. Aetiology

     Exogenous saccadic gaze  Ideal visual field 

  2. Manifestation

     Reaction time  Speed maintenance and driving quality 

  3. Limitations

     Work load  Placement 

  4. References

{{Merge to|Automotive head-up display|date=August 2018|discuss=Talk:Eyes-on-the-Road-Benefit#Untitled}}{{Multiple issues|{{Essay-like|date=June 2016}}{{weasel|date=March 2017}}}}

The Eyes-on-the-Road-Benefit (ERB), also known as the Head-Up-Display-Advantage, is the term given to the purported advantages provided to motorists when driving using a Head-Up-Display (HUD).[1] This can also be referred to as a heads-up-device or heads-up design, as compared to traditional dashboard designs, which are referred to as Head-Down-Design (HDD). A HUD is an instrument used to communicate information, such as speed, from a vehicle to the driver or pilot.[2] HUDs can either be built into a vehicle or can be bought aftermarket and installed to a vehicle’s dashboard.[3] The benefit of Eyes-on-the-Road systems stems from increased situational awareness and elimination of the need to look away from the road whilst driving, thereby increasing reaction time to external hazards, such as pedestrians.[4] There is some evidence to suggest that the scope of the ERB is limited to low cognitive load situations in which the driving task is not particularly complex.[1]

Aetiology

Research into the ERB primarily utilizes virtual reality driving simulators to mimic real life driving scenarios while eliminating situational variability. In order to examine HUDs and HDDs, studies often compare hazard reaction time, situational awareness, and quality of driving (such as speed consistency) using both systems. The extent of the ERB on different demographics, particularly those of age and experience level, are of particular interest.{{citation needed|date=March 2017}} The interaction between work-load and the influence of ERB are also frequently examined for research.

Exogenous saccadic gaze

Saccadic gaze is the perceptual mechanism through which the eye is inadvertently drawn to external stimulus without the individual's conscious action.[5] An involuntary gaze is most easily drawn by movement or distinct changes in illumination in an individual’s visual field.[6] These external stimuli can be beneficial in such situations as the movement of a pedestrian about to walk out onto the road, in turn allowing the driver to take evasive action. Exogenous cues can also be irrelevant, and often dangerous, leading to distraction from goal behaviours, such as the flashing of a cellphone taking one’s eyes off the road. By superimposing vital driving information onto the horizon in a driver’s direct line of sight, HUDS allow important exogenous cues, like the movements of other vehicles to draw the gaze of a driver whilst they monitor vital vehicle feedback such as speed or revolution count.[7] It is theorized that this can facilitate faster reaction times to hazards and improve situational awareness.

Ideal visual field

The ideal visual field is the area in which stimuli are most accurately, rapidly, and efficiently processed by the eye. In humans, this field is thought to be within 20 degrees above or below the vertical meridian of an individual’s gaze and 60 degrees either side of the horizontal meridian.[8] If an object is beyond these boundaries it will require eye movement to bring the stimuli out of periphery. By including feedback instruments in the primary field of vision, HUDs allow for the horizon and all associated stimuli to stay in the primary field vision where the information may still be processed and acknowledged by a motorist.[9]

Manifestation

Reaction time

Reaction time, and more specifically delayed reaction, is widely cited as a key contributor to vehicular accidents.[10] Reaction time in relation to the ERB is defined as the time it takes for a motorist to react to an external hazard or stimuli and then carry out the appropriate reaction, or evasive maneuver such as braking when a vehicle in front stops. The feedback offered by an HUD is projected onto the windshield of a vehicle with the aim of integrating outside stimuli and the instrumental feedback; thus removing the need to remove a driver's eyes from the road. Studies of reaction time to hazards in HUD vs HDD designs have found that the average reaction times for HUD are faster.[4] This trend appears to continue across demographics, including both categories of experience level and age.[11][12]

Speed maintenance and driving quality

Speed maintenance is the extent to which a driver maintains a speed and adjusts their speed to suit traffic laws and environmental conditions. The use of HUDs appears to produce better speed maintenance in drivers under experimental conditions when compared to HDDs.[1] It is theorized that this is because having the speedometer at the eye level of the vehicle operator allows for continuous monitoring of the vehicle’s speed. HUD use also appears to increase general driving quality, including staying within road markings, and increased smoothness of driving and navigation abilities.[2] Drivers’ capacity to focus on external cues, such as road texture, road demarcations and street signs is increased by using a seamless interface where focus on the road isn't interrupted to assess speed and other information.

Limitations

Work load

The influence of ERB on drivers is not universal. There is evidence that as the complexity of driving tasks increases, the benefits of using a HUD are decreased, and in some circumstances, they are no longer statistically significant. The ERB is diminished, for example, when individuals are driving cognitively demanding vehicles, such as industrial vehicles, or when they are asked to multitask while driving.[1] One study has shown that when placed in a cognitively demanding condition, individuals shift their focus from the road alone to focus on other tasks such as shifting gears or talking to others. Subsequently, a driver’s ability to process HUD feedback requires diversion of attention, much akin to that which occurs whilst using a HDD.[1]

Placement

There are limitations for where a HUD can be placed or projected in a vehicle before it begins to diminish the ERB and becomes more of a distraction. HUDs can be constructed so that the instrumental feedback appears to be projected out into the horizon, rather than displayed directly on the windshield.[13] In test situations, a projected HUD which appears near the nose of the vehicle is said to result in the most rapid response times and best situational awareness on the part of the driver, as well as facilitating better driving quality.[13]

References

1. ^{{Cite journal|last=Liu|first=Yung-Ching|last2=Wen|first2=Ming-Hui|date=2004-11-01|title=Comparison of head-up display (HUD) vs. head-down display (HDD): driving performance of commercial vehicle operators in Taiwan|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1071581904000497|journal=International Journal of Human-Computer Studies|volume=61|issue=5|pages=679–697|doi=10.1016/j.ijhcs.2004.06.002}}
2. ^{{Cite book|last=Harrison|first=Beverly L.|last2=Ishii|first2=Hiroshi|last3=Vicente|first3=Kim J.|last4=Buxton|first4=William A. S.|date=1995-01-01|title=Transparent Layered User Interfaces: An Evaluation of a Display Design to Enhance Focused and Divided Attention|journal=Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems|series=CHI '95|location=New York, NY, USA|publisher=ACM Press/Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.|pages=317–324|doi=10.1145/223904.223945|isbn=978-0201847055}}
3. ^{{Cite book|chapter-url=http://papers.sae.org/880217/|doi=10.4271/880217|access-date=2016-06-06|date=February 1988|last1=Suzuki|first1=Yoshiyuki|title=SAE Technical Paper Series|volume=1|last2=Otsuka|first2=Tatsumi|last3=Lino|first3=Tadashi|chapter=Development of Heads-Up Display for a Motor Vehicle}}
4. ^{{Cite book|chapter-url=http://papers.sae.org/2008-01-0203/|doi=10.4271/2008-01-0203|access-date=2016-06-06|date=2008-04-14|last1=Vlachos|first1=George|title=SAE Technical Paper Series|volume=1|last2=Papanastasiou|first2=Stylianos|last3=Charissis|first3=Vassilis|chapter=Comparative Study of Prototype Automotive HUD vs. HDD: Collision Avoidance Simulation and Results}}
5. ^{{Cite journal|last=Murray|first=David W.|last2=Bradshaw|first2=Kevin J.|last3=McLauchlan|first3=Philip F.|last4=Reid|first4=Ian D.|last5=Sharkey|first5=Paul M.|date=1995-11-01|title=Driving saccade to pursuit using image motion|journal=International Journal of Computer Vision|language=en|volume=16|issue=3|pages=205–228|doi=10.1007/BF01539627|issn=0920-5691}}
6. ^{{Cite journal|last=Trappenberg|first=Thomas P.|last2=Dorris|first2=Michael C.|last3=Munoz|first3=Douglas P.|last4=Klein|first4=Raymond M.|date=2001-02-01|title=A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|volume=13|issue=2|pages=256–271|doi=10.1162/089892901564306|issn=0898-929X}}
7. ^{{Cite book|last=Charissis|first=V.|last2=Naef|first2=M.|date=2007-06-01|title=Evaluation of Prototype Automotive Head-Up Display Interface: Testing Driver's Focusing Ability through a VR Simulation|url=http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/lpdocs/epic03/wrapper.htm?arnumber=4290174|journal=2007 IEEE Intelligent Vehicles Symposium|pages=560–565|doi=10.1109/IVS.2007.4290174|isbn=978-1-4244-1067-5}}
8. ^{{Cite journal|last=Curcio|first=Christine A.|last2=Allen|first2=Kimberly A.|date=1990-10-01|title=Topography of ganglion cells in human retina|journal=The Journal of Comparative Neurology|language=en|volume=300|issue=1|pages=5–25|doi=10.1002/cne.903000103|pmid=2229487|issn=1096-9861}}
9. ^{{Cite journal|last=Underwood|first=Geoffrey|last2=Chapman|first2=Peter|last3=Brocklehurst|first3=Neil|last4=Underwood|first4=Jean|last5=Crundall|first5=David|date=2003-01-01|title=Visual attention while driving: sequences of eye fixations made by experienced and novice drivers|journal=Ergonomics|volume=46|issue=6|pages=629–646|doi=10.1080/0014013031000090116|issn=0014-0139|pmid=12745692}}
10. ^{{Cite journal|last=Harbluk|first=Joanne L.|last2=Noy|first2=Y. Ian|last3=Trbovich|first3=Patricia L.|last4=Eizenman|first4=Moshe|date=2007-03-01|title=An on-road assessment of cognitive distraction: Impacts on drivers' visual behavior and braking performance|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0001457506001485|journal=Accident Analysis & Prevention|volume=39|issue=2|pages=372–379|doi=10.1016/j.aap.2006.08.013}}
11. ^{{Cite journal|last=Travis|first=David|date=1993-07-01|title=The human factors perspective|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/014193829390040C|journal=Displays|volume=14|issue=3|pages=178–181|doi=10.1016/0141-9382(93)90040-C}}
12. ^{{Cite journal|last=Wolffsohn|first=J. S|last2=McBrien|first2=N. A|last3=Edgar|first3=G. K|last4=Stout|first4=T|date=1998-05-01|title=The influence of cognition and age on accommodation, detection rate and response times when using a car head-up display (HUD)|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S027554089700094X|journal=Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics|volume=18|issue=3|pages=243–253|doi=10.1016/S0275-5408(97)00094-X}}
13. ^{{Cite journal|last=Smith|first=Shana|last2=Fu|first2=Shih-Hang|date=2011-04-01|title=The relationships between automobile head-up display presentation images and drivers' Kansei|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0141938210000946|journal=Displays|volume=32|issue=2|pages=58–68|doi=10.1016/j.displa.2010.12.001}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Eyes-on-the-Road-Benefit}}

1 : Vehicle technology

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/11 1:40:08