词条 | Behavioral neuroscience |
释义 |
HistoryBehavioral neuroscience as a scientific discipline emerged from a variety of scientific and philosophical traditions in the 18th and 19th centuries. In philosophy, people like René Descartes proposed physical models to explain animal and human behavior. Descartes, for example, suggested that the pineal gland, a midline unpaired structure in the brain of many organisms, was the point of contact between mind and body. Descartes also elaborated on a theory in which the pneumatics of bodily fluids could explain reflexes and other motor behavior. This theory was inspired by moving statues in a garden in Paris.[4] Other philosophers also helped give birth to psychology. One of the earliest textbooks in the new field, The Principles of Psychology by William James, argues that the scientific study of psychology should be grounded in an understanding of biology: {{quote|Bodily experiences, therefore, and more particularly brain-experiences, must take a place amongst those conditions of the mental life of which Psychology need take account. The spiritualist and the associationist must both be 'cerebralists,' to the extent at least of admitting that certain peculiarities in the way of working of their own favorite principles are explicable only by the fact that the brain laws are a codeterminant of their result.Our first conclusion, then, is that a certain amount of brain-physiology must be presupposed or included in Psychology.[5]}} The emergence of both psychology and behavioral neuroscience as legitimate sciences can be traced from the emergence of physiology from anatomy, particularly neuroanatomy. Physiologists conducted experiments on living organisms, a practice that was distrusted by the dominant anatomists of the 18th and 19th centuries.[6] The influential work of Claude Bernard, Charles Bell, and William Harvey helped to convince the scientific community that reliable data could be obtained from living subjects. Even before the 18th and 19th century, behavioral neuroscience was beginning to take form as far back as 1700 B.C.[7] The question that seems to continually arise is: what is the connection between the mind and body? The debate is formally referred to as the mind-body problem. There are two major schools of thought that attempt to resolve the mind–body problem; monism and dualism.[4] Plato and Aristotle are two of several philosophers who participated in this debate. Plato believed that the brain was where all mental thought and processes happened.[7] In contrast, Aristotle believed that the brain served the purpose of cooling down the emotions derived from the heart.[4] The mind-body problem was a stepping stone toward attempting to understand the connection between the mind and body. Another debate arose about was localization of function or functional specialization versus equipotentiality which played a significant role in the development in behavioral neuroscience. As a result of localization of function research, many famous people found within psychology have come to various different conclusions. Wilder Penfield was able to develop a map of the cerebral cortex through studying epileptic patients along with Rassmussen.[4] Research on localization of function has led behavioral neuroscientist to a better understanding of which parts of the brain control behavior. This is best exemplified through the case study of Phineas Gage. The term "psychobiology" has been used in a variety of contexts, emphasizing the importance of biology, which is the discipline that studies organic, neural and cellular modifications in behavior, plasticity in neuroscience, and biological diseases in all aspects, in addition, biology focuses and analyzes behavior and all the subjects it is concerned about, from a scientific point of view. In this context, psychology helps as a complementary, but important discipline in the neurobiological sciences. The role of psychology in this questions is that of a social tool that backs up the main or strongest biological science. The term "psychobiology" was first used in its modern sense by Knight Dunlap in his book An Outline of Psychobiology (1914).[8] Dunlap also was the founder and editor-in-chief of the journal Psychobiology. In the announcement of that journal, Dunlap writes that the journal will publish research "...bearing on the interconnection of mental and physiological functions", which describes the field of behavioral neuroscience even in its modern sense.[8] Relationship to other fields of psychology and biologyIn many cases, humans may serve as experimental subjects in behavioral neuroscience experiments; however, a great deal of the experimental literature in behavioral neuroscience comes from the study of non-human species, most frequently rats, mice, and monkeys. As a result, a critical assumption in behavioral neuroscience is that organisms share biological and behavioral similarities, enough to permit extrapolations across species. This allies behavioral neuroscience closely with comparative psychology, evolutionary psychology, evolutionary biology, and neurobiology. Behavioral neuroscience also has paradigmatic and methodological similarities to neuropsychology, which relies heavily on the study of the behavior of humans with nervous system dysfunction (i.e., a non-experimentally based biological manipulation). Synonyms for behavioral neuroscience include biopsychology, biological psychology, and psychobiology.[9] Physiological psychology is a subfield of behavioral neuroscience, with an appropriately narrower definition Research methodsThe distinguishing characteristic of a behavioral neuroscience experiment is that either the independent variable of the experiment is biological, or some dependent variable is biological. In other words, the nervous system of the organism under study is permanently or temporarily altered, or some aspect of the nervous system is measured (usually to be related to a behavioral variable). Disabling or decreasing neural function
Enhancing neural function
Measuring neural activity
Genetic techniques
Other research methodsComputational models - Using a computer to formulate real-world problems to develop solutions.[27] Although this method is often focused in computer science, it has begun to move towards other areas of study.For example, psychology is one of these areas. Computational models allow researchers in psychology to enhance their understanding of the functions and developments in nervous systems. Examples of methods include the modelling of neurons, networks and brain systems and theoretical analysis.[28] Computational methods have a wide variety of roles including clarifying experiments, hypothesis testing and generating new insights. These techniques play an increasing role in the advancement of biological psychology.[29] Limitations and advantagesDifferent manipulations have advantages and limitations. Neural tissue destroyed as a primary consequence of a surgery, electric shock or neurotoxin can confound the results so that the physical trauma masks changes in the fundamental neurophysiological processes of interest. For example, when using an electrolytic probe to create a purposeful lesion in a distinct region of the rat brain, surrounding tissue can be affected: so, a change in behavior exhibited by the experimental group post-surgery is to some degree a result of damage to surrounding neural tissue, rather than by a lesion of a distinct brain region.[30][36] Most genetic manipulation techniques are also considered permanent.[36] Temporary lesions can be achieved with advanced in genetic manipulations, for example, certain genes can now be switched on and off with diet.[36] Pharmacological manipulations also allow blocking of certain neurotransmitters temporarily as the function returns to its previous state after the drug has been metabolized.[31] Topic areasIn general, behavioral neuroscientists study similar themes and issues as academic psychologists, though limited by the need to use nonhuman animals. As a result, the bulk of literature in behavioral neuroscience deals with mental processes and behaviors that are shared across different animal models such as:
However, with increasing technical sophistication and with the development of more precise noninvasive methods that can be applied to human subjects, behavioral neuroscientists are beginning to contribute to other classical topic areas of psychology, philosophy, and linguistics, such as:
Behavioral neuroscience has also had a strong history of contributing to the understanding of medical disorders, including those that fall under the purview of clinical psychology and biological psychopathology (also known as abnormal psychology). Although animal models do not exist for all mental illnesses, the field has contributed important therapeutic data on a variety of conditions, including:
AwardsNobel LaureatesThe following Nobel Prize winners could reasonably be considered behavioral neuroscientists or neurobiologists.{{By whom|date=October 2017}} (This list omits winners who were almost exclusively neuroanatomists or neurophysiologists; i.e., those that did not measure behavioral or neurobiological variables.) {{columns-list|colwidth=30em|
}}Kavli Prize in Neuroscience
See also{{Portal|Neuroscience}}{{columns-list|colwidth=30em|
}} References1. ^Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, Biological Psychology: An Introduction to Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience, 6/e, {{ISBN|978-0-87893-705-9}}, p. 2 2. ^Psychobiology, Merriam-Webster's Online Dictionary 3. ^Thomas, R.K. 1993, "INTRODUCTION: A Biopsychology Festschrift in Honor of Lelon J. Peacock", Journal of General Psychology, vol. 120, no. 1, pp. 5. 4. ^1 2 3 {{cite book |last=Carlson |first=Neil|title=Physiology of Behavior (9th Ed.) |publisher=Allyn and Bacon |pages=11–14 |year=2007 |isbn=0-205-46724-5}} 5. ^{{cite book |last=James |first=William |title=The Principles of Psychology, Vol. One |publisher=Dover Publications, Inc. |pages=4–5 |year=1950/1890 |isbn=0-486-20381-6}} 6. ^{{cite book |last=Shepherd |first=Gordon M.|title=Foundations of the Neuron Doctrine |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=1991 |isbn=0-19-506491-7}} 7. ^1 {{cite web|url=http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/courses/1010/mangels/neuro/history/history.html |title=History of Neuroscience |publisher=Columbia University |accessdate=2014-05-04}} 8. ^1 {{cite journal |last=Dewsbury |first=Donald |title=Psychobiology |journal=American Psychologist |volume=46 |issue=3 |pages=198–205 |year=1991 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.46.3.198 |pmid=2035930 |accessdate= }} 9. ^S. Marc Breedlove, Mark Rosenzweig and Neil V. Watson (2007). Biological Psychology: An Introduction to Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience 6e. Sinauer Associates. {{ISBN|978-0-87893-705-9}} 10. ^{{cite journal|last1=Zhu|first1=Hu|title=Silencing synapses with DREADDs|journal=Neuron|volume=82|issue=4|pages=723–725|pmc=4109642|year=2014|doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.002}} 11. ^Kim, Jeansok J.; DeCola, Joseph P.; Landeira-Fernandez, Jesus; Fanselow, Michael S. "N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist APV blocks acquisition but not expression of fear conditioning." Behavioral Neuroscience. Vol 105(1), Feb 1991, 126-133. {doi|10.1037/0735-7044.105.1.126} 12. ^Schneider et al. "Controlling Neuronal Activity." American Journal of Psychiatry 165:562, May 2008 {{doi|10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030444}} 13. ^Zhang, et al. "Multimodal fast optical interrogation of neural circuitry." Nature. Vol 446. 5 April 2007. {{doi|10.1038/nature05744}} 14. ^Chow, B. Y. et al. "High-performance genetically targetable opticalneural silencing by light-driven proton pumps." Nature. Vol 463. 7 January 2010 15. ^Gradinaru, Thompson, and Deisseroth. "eNpHR: a Natronomonas halorhodopsin enhanced for optogenetic applications." Brain cell Biology. Vol 36 (1-4). Aug 2008. {{doi|10.1007/s11068-008-9027-6}} 16. ^{{cite journal|last1=Ferguson|first1=Susan|title=Grateful DREADDs: Engineered Receptors Reveal How Neural Circuits Regulate Behavior|journal=Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews|date=2012|volume=37|pages=296–297|url=http://www.nature.com/npp/journal/v37/n1/full/npp2011179a.html}} 17. ^Zhang, Wang, Boyden, and Deisseroth. "Channelrhodopsin-2 and optical control of excitable cells." Nature Methods. VOL.3 NO.10. OCTOBER 2006 18. ^Gradinaru et al. "Molecular and Cellular Approaches for Diversifying and Extending Optogenetics." Cell. 2010. {{doi|10.1016/j.cell.2010.02.037}} 19. ^Ebner, T. J. and Chen, G. "Use of voltage-sensitive dyes and optical recordings in the central nervous system." Progress in NeurobiologyVolume 46, Issue 5, August 1995, 463-506. {{doi|10.1016/0301-0082(95)00010-S}} 20. ^Micah S. Siegel and Ehud Y. Isacoff. "A Genetically Encoded Optical Probe of Membrane Voltage."Neuron, Vol. 19, 735–741, October, 1997 21. ^O'Donovan, Hoa, Sholomenkoa, and Yeea. "Real-time imaging of neurons retrogradely and anterogradely labelled with calcium-sensitive dyes." Journal of Neuroscience Methods. Vol 46, Issue 2, February 1993, 91-106. {{doi|10.1016/0165-0270(93)90145-H}} 22. ^Nicola Heim and Oliver Griesbeck. "Genetically Encoded Indicators of Cellular Calcium Dynamics Based on Troponin C and Green Fluorescent Protein." The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 279, 14280-14286. April 2, 2004 {{doi|10.1074/jbc.M312751200}} 23. ^Gero Miesenböck, Dino A. De Angelis & James E. Rothman1. "Visualizing secretion and synaptic transmission with pH-sensitive green fluorescent proteins." Nature 394, 192-195 (9 July 1998) | {{doi|10.1038/28190}} 24. ^von Heimendahl, M., Itskov, P., Arabzadeh, E., & Diamond, M. (2007). Neuronal activity in rat barrel cortex underlying texture discrimination. PLoS Biol, 5(11), e305. 25. ^Ocampo, T., Knight, K., Dunleavy, R., & Shah, S. N. (2015). Techniques, Benefits, and Challenges of PET-MR. Radiologic technology, 86(4), 393-412. 26. ^Sanei, S., & Chambers, J. A. (2013). EEG signal processing. John Wiley & Sons. 27. ^Otago, U. o., n/d. Computational Modelling. [Online] Available at: http://www.otago.ac.nz/courses/otago032670.pdf 28. ^Churchland, P. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2016). The computational brain. MIT press. 29. ^Brodland, G. W. (2015, December). How computational models can help unlock biological systems. In Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology (Vol. 47, pp. 62-73). Academic Press. 30. ^{{cite journal|last1=Kirby|first1=Elizabeth D.|last2=Jensen|first2=Kelly|last3=Goosens|first3=Ki A.|last4=Kaufer|first4=Daniela|title=Stereotaxic Surgery for Excitotoxic Lesion of Specific Brain Areas in the Adult Rat|pmc=3476400|journal=Journal of Visualized Experiments|issue=65|pages=4079|doi=10.3791/4079|date=19 July 2012}} 31. ^1 2 3 T Abel, KM Lattal (2001) "Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval" Current Opinion in Neurobiology External links{{Spoken Wikipedia|biologicalpsychology.ogg|2006-12-18}}
3 : Behavioral neuroscience|Branches of psychology|Neuropsychology |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。