词条 | Juan Pascual-Leone | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
释义 |
| name = Juan Pascual-Leone | image = JPL CLOSE UP SMILING SPAIN ABOUT 2013-14.jpg | image_size = | birth_date = 1933 | birth_place = Spain | fields = Developmental Neuropsychology | workplaces = York University | thesis_title = Cognitive development and cognitive style: A general psychological integration. | thesis_year = 1969 | doctoral_advisor = Jean Piaget | academic_advisors = Herman Witkin | known_for = neo-Piagetian approach to cognitive development Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO) }}Juan Pascual-Leone (born 1933 in Spain) is a developmental neuropsychologist and the founder of the neo-Piagetian approach to cognitive development. He introduced this term to the literature[1] and put forward[2] key neo-Piagetian predictions about mental attention and working memory in the context of developmental growth.[3][4] Pascual-Leone pioneered descriptions of developmental cognitive growth from an organismic perspective, i.e. "from within" the subjects' task processing.[5][4] He calls this perspective "metasubjective", and contrasts it with the external observer's perspective. Metasubjective modeling requires organismic-causal task-analysis.[6][8][7] Using this method he clarified distinctions between learning (including the learning of executive functions), maturational-developmental processes, and working memory, studying their interrelationships from a constructivist-developmental organismic viewpoint. His theoretical-and-empirical methods of research and constructivist views on scientific epistemology led to his propounding the Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO), his general neuropsychological causal model framed in terms of organismic operators, schemes, and principles. EducationPascual-Leone studied medicine at the University of Valencia, specializing in psychiatry and neurology in Santander, on the north coast of Spain, and in Paris. His background as a medical doctor and neuropsychiatrist, and his experience studying psychology with Jean Piaget, contributed to a sophisticated understanding of Piaget's theory; and led to his development of the theory of constructive operators – an organismic-causal expansion and reformulation that integrates the ides of, among others, his mentors Piaget and Herman Witkin ideas. Under the Supervision of PiagetIn 1963–69, Pascual-Leone studied psychology at the University of Geneva, Switzerland, where, in 1964, he obtained his M.A. (Licence) in Experimental Child Development, and, in 1969, his Ph.D. in psychology. Here, he was under the direct supervision of Jean Piaget (1896-1980), at the peak of his fame as child psychologist and constructivist-development researcher, whom he refers to as "my intellectual father in Psychology".[8] As one of the later graduate students of Piaget, he obtained first-hand knowledge of Piaget's theory, collaborating in Piaget's book on mental image. He was one of the first Piaget's students to explicitly highlight shortcomings of the master's theory. Under the Supervision of WitkinPascual-Leone also studied under the American psychologist Herman Witkin (1916-1979). Witkin—a student of Max Wertheimer, a founder of Gestalt Psychology—conducted organismic-experimental research on individual differences (cognitive styles) in cognitive and perceptional psychology as well as personality development; his focus was on psycho-social processes and cognitive differentiation.[9] Witkin was an innovator who pioneered, from an organismic perspective, theories of cognitive styles, psychological differentiation, and learning styles.[10] Pascual-Leone defended his doctoral thesis in psychology[13] in Geneva, with Piaget and Witkin as supervisors. In 1964–65, Pascual-Leone's did research at Witkin's laboratory at the State University of New York, Downstate Medical Center. This final doctoral research was done under Witkin's sponsordship and supervision with the help of a postdoctoral fellowship from the Foundations Fund for Research in Psychiatry. Working with Witkin influenced Pascual-Leone's later TCO theory, which was more process-analytical and developmental but in line with Witkin's theory.[8] Early research into cognitive developmentPascual-Leone's now-classic cognitive developmental research in the 1960s led to his seminal paper in 1970,[2] one of the 500 most cited papers in the field of psychology. In this work he proposed a mathematical model of endogenous mental-attention, and explained how it develops as a function of chronological age in normal children. His findings demonstrated for the first time that, when measured behaviorally and via task analysis, children's mental-attentional capacity increases after the age of three, by one symbol-processing unit every other year until it reaches seven units at 15–16 years. Seven units, according to Miller[11] and Pascual-Leone,[2][12] is the maximum-capacity of mental/executive attention in adults (although the habitual mental-attention level of adults tends to be about 5 units). His mental-attention model was the first to quantify the effective complexity of processing stages in human development. Many of Pascual-Leone's ideas challenged the scientific establishment at the time, and many experts claim his theories continue to do so. Commenting on this work, Barrouillet and Gaillard wrote: "Neo-Piagetian theories have the potential to account for most of the learning difficulties and developmental disorders by cumulating the strength of the functionalist and the structuralist approaches."[13] Quantifying levels of cognitive developmentPascual-Leone analyzed developmental data in terms of a task's constructive complexity. He assessed complexity by number of essential operators, relations, or schemes that children must simultaneously relate to produce a performance. In 1963, he inferred with his analyses that there is a maximum mental demand that each child's age group can cope with – the characteristic mental (M-) power of each developmental stage-level.[2] Only when the growth of mental power in a child is equal to or larger than the task's mental demand, can the child reliably solve a task. Today this information is well recognized; but in the nineteen seventies and eighties the idea was not taken seriously. Pascual-Leone was the first to claim, via his task analyses, that the true organismic stages were in fact the sub-stages of Piaget.[8] Recently Arsalidou, Pascual-Leone, Johnson, Morris, and Taylor have produced data suggesting that these levels of functioning may be expressed in adults by incrementation of brain activity in the executive (prefrontal lobes, etc.) network.[22] Later workIn later years, Pascual-Leone's scientific work received increased recognition.[14][15][16][17][18][19] His theory has been validated in his laboratory[20][7][21][22][23][24][25] and by independent researchers, who, often without explicit reference to Pascual-Leone, have supported his original predictions and subsequent results, either using his tasks or methods that converge with his.[26][27][28][29][30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37] The impact of Pascual-Leone's work can be seen in three scholarly controversies: (1) the debate over Trabasso's critique of Pascual-Leone's 1970 paper;[2][38][39][40][41] (2) The multi-author discussion of Kemps, De Rammelaere, and Desmet[42] comparing Pascual-Leone's model of mental attention with Baddeley's model of working memory;[43][44][45] and (3) The discussion of Demetriou, Spanoudis, and Shayer's study on speed of processing, working memory, and general intelligence.[46][5] A further indicator of the interest in Pascual-Leone's work is evidenced by various published interviews.[8][47][48][49][50] Cognitive stylesJuan Pascual-Leone was among the earlier developmental researchers to emphasize the role of cognitive styles (e.g., field-dependence-independence, adaptive flexibility) and individual difference in cognitive development;[51][52] and he was the first to quantify mental-attention capacity throughout development, which is important because mental attention is a causal maturational component of working memory. His research on children's and adults' mental-attentional capacity and on cognitive styles (which affect the person's propensity to experience and cope with misleading/conflicting situations versus facilitating situations), as well as his metasubjective ("from within") analysis, has opened up new perspectives for understanding cognitive processes, and has helped to clarify causal-developmental relations between affect/motivation, cognition, individual differences, and complex/conceptual processes. Pascual-Leone's work can be classified under four diverse categories: (1) Dialectical constructivist epistemology, the theory of constructive operators, a model for quantifying mental/executive attention, and process task analysis; (2) Educational domains: math, visuospatial, logic, language, science education, giftedness; (3) Individual differences, cognitive styles, developmental neuropsychology, and brain semantics; and (4) Mental health: psychotherapy,[53][54] meditation, and human change. Regarding his innovative work on individual differences as sources of difficulty/conflict in Piagetian tasks, Case and Edelstein have written:
Pascual-Leone published two early books in collaboration, but is now completing, with Janice M. Johnson, an ambitious book of synthesis. He has published numerous articles and chapters (some of which can be found in [https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Juan_Pascual-Leone ResearchGate]). In 2006, he received an honorary doctorate from the University of Cyprus. The theory of constructive operators (TCO)Pascual-Leone proposed the Theory of Constructive Operators (TCO) – an organismic cognitive-developmental theory that is neuropsychologically interpretable in terms of physiological brain processes.[57][58][59] He was the first to quantify cognitive-processing limitations in novel problem-solving situations and at different stages of development, and was the first to use a formal, explicit method of process-task analysis to estimate with some quantitative accuracy the mental-attention demands of tasks.[60] This theory explains human psychological functioning as the product of the dynamic combination of schemes (the brain's "software" – information/action bearing functional-system processes, embodied by cell assemblies or networks) and "hidden hardware" operators of the brain. Operators acting on schemes generate thoughts, actions, and learning. Hidden operators are brain-resource mechanisms that regulate functioning of (and can change) schemes. Piaget left these resources unexplained and often referred to them as principles like "regulations", "accommodation", and "equilibration". Operators intervene in all neuropsychological processes that are emergent and not automatized or overlearned: M-operator (mental-attention capacity, which explains the innate-developmental basis of working memory); I-operator (attention-inhibition mechanism causing attentional interruption); L-operator (i.e., logical- structural learning capability); C-learning (content-learning capability); F-operator (i.e., the neo-Gestaltist "field effects", or "minimum principle", or "S-R compatibility"); etc. Table 1 shows different operators of the TCO and some of their key brain regions.[74][61] Pascual-Leone's model of mental attention includes activation and inhibition processes. The activation component (M capacity) boosts action/representation schemes that should control task performance (according to affects and/or executive schemes). M-operator capacity is measured in terms of the maximum number of mental schemes (not strongly activated by the input, learning, or by strong affects) that can be boosted into working memory (i.e., mental focus or attentional centration) at any one time. The method of M-measurement evaluates cognitive demands of a task in order to predict age at which a participant can successfully complete it. By refining the method and accumulating research evidence, Pascual-Leone reached interval scales of measurement of this mental-attentional load. Pascual-Leone also highlighted brain regions that sub-serve cognitive processes linked to his theoretical constructs, offering a tentative chronological map of their evolution.[62][63] M capacity is the key maturational causal factor of working memory. Working memory refers to all the schemes in a person's repertoire that are sufficiently activated (irrespective of the cause) to co-determine the ongoing process of representation or performance. There are other causes of scheme activation (including affects/emotions, overlearning/automatization, field factors); for this reason the size of working memory is often larger than the size of M-centration.[64][12][62] Table 1. Some of TCO's hidden operators listed in a plausible order of evolutionary emergence
Current workPascual-Leone is now professor emeritus and senior scholar at York University, Toronto, but continues to have an active laboratory at York University. He has completed with Dr. Janice Johnson, and is now analyzing, a longitudinal developmental study of mental attentional capacity. He holds, with Dr. Marie Arsalidou, a research grant from Canada's National Science and Engineering Research Council (NSERC). This work involves further study of mental-attention measurement using a modified n-back paradigm that is applicable across content domains (Color-Matching Task, Letter-Matching Task, Number Matching Task, etc.). These tasks are being compared with established mental-capacity tasks (such as Figural Intersections Task (FIT)) and are related to language (e.g., bilingualism) and other domains. The same NSERC project will investigate brain correlates of the tasks in children by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In a different direction, Pascual-Leone's laboratory is studying how to energize a person's functional mental attention (not his/her maturational capacity or reserve) by using meditation methods. His lab has previously shown that regular practice of Tai Chi increases the functional level of mental attention.[65] References1. ^Pascual-Leone, J., & Smith, J. (1969). "The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: A new experimental paradigm and a neo-Piagetian model". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 328-355. {{authority control}}{{DEFAULTSORT:Pascual Leone, Juan}}2. ^1 2 3 4 Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). "A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages." Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345. 3. ^Pascual-Leone, J., (2011). "[https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Juan_Pascual-Leone/publication/232503406_A_dialectical_constructivist_approach_to_experiential_change/links/54b563510cf2318f0f97f27a Piaget as a pioneer of dialectical constructivism: Seeking dynamic processes for human science.]" In E. Marti & C. Rodriguez (Eds.), After Piaget. (pp. 15-41) Edison, NJ: Transaction Publishers. 4. ^1 Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (in press). "[https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Juan_Pascual-Leone/publication/266142426_ORGANISMIC_CAUSAL_MODELS_FROM_WITHIN_CLARIFY_DEVELOPMENTAL_CHANGE_AND_STAGES/links/54270f640cf26120b7b3480e.pdf Organismic causal models "from within" clarify developmental change and stages]". In N. Budwig, E. Turiel, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), New perspectives on human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 5. ^1 Pascual-Leone, J. (2013). "Can we model organismic causes of working memory, efficiency and fluid intelligence? A meta-subjective perspective". Intelligence, 41, 738-743. 6. ^Pascual-Leone, LOGIC 7. ^1 Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2010). "Misleading cues improve developmental assessment of attentional capacity: The colour matching task". Cognitive Development, 25, 262-277. 8. ^1 2 3 Cardellini, L., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2004). "On cognitive mentors, cognitive development, education, and constructivism: An interview with Juan Pascual-Leone". Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 4, 199-219. 9. ^Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R.B., Faterson, H. G., Goodenough, D.R., & Karp, S.A. (1962). Psychological differentiation: Studies of development. New York, NY: Wiley. 10. ^Witkin, H. & Goodenough, D. R. (1981) Cognitive style: essence and origin. New York: International Universities Press (Psychological Issues # 51). 11. ^{{Cite journal | last1 = Miller | first1 = G. A. | authorlink = George Armitage Miller| title = The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information | doi = 10.1037/h0043158 | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 63 | issue = 2 | pages = 81–97 | year = 1956 | pmid = 13310704| url = http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Miller/| pmc = | citeseerx = 10.1.1.308.8071 }} 12. ^1 Pascual-Leone, J. (1984). "Attention, dialectic, and mental effort: Towards an organismic theory of life stages". In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & G. Armon (Eds.) Beyond formal operations: Late adolescence and adult cognitive development (pp. 182-215). NY: Praeger. 13. ^Barrouillet, P., & Gaillard, V. (2011a). "Advances and issues: Some thoughts about controversial questions". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian and cognitive approaches (pp. 263-271). New York: Psychology Press. p. 270 14. ^Bideau, J., Houde, O., & Pedinielli, J-L. (1995). L'homme en developpement. Paris: PUF. 15. ^Lautrey, J. (Ed.) (2006). Psychologie du développement et psychologie différentielle (pp. 76-102). Paris: PUF. 16. ^Morra, S., Gobbo, C., Marini, Z., & Sheese, R. (2008). Cognitive development: Neo-Piagetian perspectives. New York, NY: Erlbaum. 17. ^de Ribaupierre, A, (2016) "Memoire de travail, developpement cognitif et performances scolaires". In P-A. Doudin & E. Tardif (Eds.) Neurosciences et cognition: Perspectives pour les sciences de l'education. (pp. 165-188). Louvain-la-Neuve – Belgique: De Boeck, superieur. 18. ^Troadec, B., & Martinot, C. (2003). Le developpment cognitif: Theories actuelles de la pensee en contextes. Paris: Belin. 19. ^Vuyk, R. (1981) Overview and critique of Piaget's genetic epistemology 1965-1980 (Vol. II). London, U.K: Academic Press. 20. ^Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). "Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 286-305. 21. ^Balioussis, C., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2012) "Fluency and complexity in children's writing: The role of mental attention and executive function". Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata / Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 33-45. 22. ^Burtis, P. J. (1982). "Capacity increase and chunking in the development of short-term memory". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 387-413. 23. ^Howard, S. J., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2013). "Measurement of mental attention: Assessing a cognitive component underlying performance on standardized intelligence tests". Psychological Assessment and Test Modeling, 55(3), 250-272. 24. ^Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). "Processing limitation in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function". Child Development, 77, 1822-1841. 25. ^Johnson, J., Fabian, V., & Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). "Quantitative hardware-stages that constrain language development". Human Development, 32, 245-271. 26. ^Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1979). "Pascual-Leone's M construct as a link between cognitive-developmental and psychometric concepts of intelligence". Intelligence, 3, 41-63. 27. ^Onwumere, O., & Reid, N. (2014). "Field dependency and performance in mathematics". European Journal of Educational Research, 3, 43-57. 28. ^Cowan, N. (2014). "Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education". Educational Psychology Review, 26, 197-223. 29. ^Cowan, N. (2016). "Working memory maturation: Can we get at the essence of cognitive growth?" Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 239-264. 30. ^Case, R. (1998). "The development of conceptual structures". In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.; pp. 745-800). New York, NY: Wiley. 31. ^Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). "The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1-2, Serial No. 246). 32. ^Niaz, M. (2006). "Can the study of thermochemistry facilitate students' differentiation between heat energy and temperature?" Journal of Science Education and Technology, 15, 269-276. 33. ^Mella, N., Fagot, D. Lecerf, T. & de Ribaupierre, A. (2014) "Working memory and intraindividual variability in processing speed: A lifespan developmental and individual-differences study". Memory & Cognition, vol. 42, 8, November. doi: [https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1 10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1]. 34. ^Morra, S. Camba, R., Guglielmo Calvini, G & Bracco, F. (2013) "Italians do it better? M-capacity measurement and cross-linguistic differences in the Direction Following Task (DFT)". Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata / Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, xiii, 1/2013. 35. ^Morra, S & Muscella, L. (2016) "A reappraisal of the Bose-Einstein model of the CSVI task : New experimental evidence, replications, and implications". EWOMS-8 - Liège, September 1, 2016. 36. ^de Ribaupierre, A., Fagot, D., & Lecerf, T. (2011). "Working memory capacity and its role in cognitive development". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory (pp. 105-133). New York, NY: Psychology Press. 37. ^Tsaparlis, G. & Angelopoulus, V. (2000). "A model of problem-solving: Its operation, validity, and its usefulness in the case of organic-synthesis problems". Journal of Science Education, 84,131–153. 38. ^Pascual-Leone, J. (1978). "Compounds, confounds and models in developmental information processing: A reply to Trabasso and Foellinger". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 18-40. 39. ^Pascual-Leone, J., & Sparkman, E. (1980). "The dialectics of empiricism and rationalism: A last methodological reply to Trabasso". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 88-101. 40. ^Trabasso, T. (1978). "On the estimation of parameters and the evaluation of a mathematical model: A reply to Pascual-Leone". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 41-45. 41. ^Trabasso, T., & Foellinger, D. B. (1978). "Information processing capacity in children: A test of Pascual-Leone's model". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 1-17. 42. ^Kemps, E., De Rammelaere, S., & Desmet, T. (2000). "The development of working memory: Exploring the complementarity of two models". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 89-109. 43. ^Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2000). "Development of working memory: Should the Pascual-Leone and the Baddeley and Hitch models be merged?" Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 128-137. 44. ^Pascual-Leone, J. (2000). "Reflections on working memory: Are the two models complementary?" Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 138-154. 45. ^de Ribaupierre, A., & Bailleux, C. (2000). "The development of working memory: Further note on the comparability of two models of working memory". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 110-127. 46. ^Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., & Shayer, M. (2013). "Developing intelligence: Is a comprehensive theory possible?" Intelligence, 41, 730-731. 47. ^Corral, A. & del Valle, Ch. & Pascual-Leone, J. (1993). "Entrevista con Pascual-Leone: Sobre inteligencia artificial, creatividad, inteligencia verdadera, voluntad, aprendizaje y desarrollo". Tarbiya: Revista del Instituto de Ciencias de la Educación. Universidad Antonoma de Madrid, 515-27. 48. ^Delval, J., with Pascual-Leone, J. (1977). "Entrevista con Juan Pascual-Leone". Cuadernos de Psicologia (Madrid, Editorial Fundamentos), 8-9, 26-38. 49. ^Society for Research in Child Development oral history project 50. ^Canadian Psychological Association oral history project 51. ^Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). "An organismic process model of Witkin's field-dependence-independence". In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36-70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 52. ^Shafrir, U., & Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). "Postfailure reflectivity/impulsivity and spontaneous attention to errors". Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 378-387. 53. ^Pascual-Leone, J. (1991). "Emotions, development and psychotherapy: A dialectical constructivist perspective". In J. Safran & L. Greenberg (Eds.), Emotion, psychotherapy and change (pp. 302-335). New York: Guilford. 54. ^Greenberg, L., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2001). "A dialectical constructivist view of the creation of personal meaning". Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 14, 165-186. 55. ^1 Pascual-Leone, J. (1969) Cognitive development and cognitive style: A general psychological integration. Ph.D. Thesis presented at the Faculty of Psychology and Science of Education. University of Geneva, Thesis No. 44, Geneva, Switzerland. 56. ^Case, R., & Edelstein, W. (Eds.). (1993). The new structuralism in cognitive development: Theory and research on individual pathways. Basel, Switzerland: Karger. p. 5-6 57. ^Pascual-Leone, J. (1995). "[https://www.karger.com/Article/Pdf/278340 Learning and development as dialectical factors in cognitive growth]". Human Development, 38, 338-348. 58. ^1 Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., Morris, D., & Taylor, M. J. (2013). "A balancing act of the brain: Activations and deactivations driven by cognitive load". Brain and Behavior. {{doi|10.1002/brb3.128}}. 59. ^Pascual-Leone, J., Pascual-Leone, A., & Arsalidou, M. (2015). "[https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences/article/div-classtitleneuropsychology-still-needs-to-model-organismic-processes-from-withindiv/8C30D36323DE208CC43609DC2CA4E1B3# Neuropsychology still needs to model organismic processes 'from within']". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e83. 60. ^1 Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2011). "A developmental theory of mental attention: Its applications to measurement and task analysis". In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian and cognitive approaches (pp. 13-46). New York: Psychology Press. 61. ^Pascual-Leone J. & Johnson, J. (book in preparation). The working mind #1: Meaning and mental attention in human development. 62. ^1 2 Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2005). "A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence". In O. Wilhelm & R. Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 177-201). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 63. ^Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., & Agostino, A. (2010). "Mental attention, multiplicative structures, and the causal problem of cognitive development". In M. Ferrari & L. Vuletic (Eds.), Developmental interplay between mind, brain and education: Essays in honor of Robbie Case (pp. 49-82). New York, NY: Springer. 64. ^Johnson, J., Im-Bolter, N., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2003). "Development of mental attention in gifted and mainstream children: The role of mental capacity, inhibition, and speed of processing". Child Development, 74, 1594-1614. 65. ^Kim, T., Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J. & Tamim, H. (2016). "The mental-attention Tai Chi effect with older adults". BMC Psychology, 4:29. 4 : 1933 births|Living people|Neuropsychologists|York University faculty |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。