请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Biorhythm
释义

  1. Hypothesis

     Calculation 

  2. History

  3. Critical views

      Additional Studies Supporting the Classification of Biorhythms as Pseudoscientific    Prytula and Colleagues (1980)[17]    James (1984)[18]    Peveto (1980)[19]  

  4. See also

  5. References

  6. Further reading

     Books  Research publications 
{{About|the pseudoscientific beliefs|the scientific study of biological cycles|Chronobiology}}{{Multiple issues|{{weasel|date=April 2017}}{{more footnotes|date=July 2009}}
}}{{Alternative medicine sidebar}}

A biorhythm (from Greek βίος - bios, "life"[1] and ῥυθμός - rhuthmos, "any regular recurring motion, rhythm"[2]) is an attempt to predict various aspects of a person's life through simple mathematical cycles. The theory was developed by Wilhelm Fliess in the late 19th century, and was popularized in the United States in late 1970s. Most scientists believe that the idea has no more predictive power than chance.[3] "The theory of biorhythms is a theory that claims our daily lives are significantly affected by rhythmic cycles."[4][5][6]

Hypothesis

Basic rhythm details
  • Physical cycle
    • 23 days; Circavigintan
    • coordination
    • strength
    • well-being
  • Emotional cycle
    • 28 days; Circatrigintan
    • creativity
    • sensitivity
    • mood
    • perception
    • awareness
  • Intellectual cycle
    • 33 days; Circatrigintan
    • alertness
    • analytical functioning
    • logical analysis
    • memory or recall
    • communication

According to the theory of biorhythms, a person's life is influenced by rhythmic biological cycles that affect his or her ability in various domains, such as mental, physical and emotional activity. These cycles begin at birth and oscillate in a steady (sine wave) fashion throughout life, and by modeling them mathematically, it is suggested that a person's level of ability in each of these domains can be predicted from day to day. The theory is built on the idea that the biofeedback chemical and hormonal secretion functions within the body could show a sinusoidal behavior over time.

Most biorhythm models use three cycles: a 23-day physical cycle, a 28-day emotional cycle, and a 33-day intellectual cycle.[8] Although the 28-day cycle is the same length as the average woman's menstrual cycle and was originally described as a "female" cycle (see below), the two are not necessarily in synchronization. Each of these cycles varies between high and low extremes sinusoidally, with days where the cycle crosses the zero line described as "critical days" of greater risk or uncertainty.

The numbers from +100% (maximum) to -100% (minimum) indicate where on each cycle the rhythms are on a particular day. In general, a rhythm at 0% is crossing the midpoint and is thought to have no real impact on your life, whereas a rhythm at +100% (at the peak of that cycle) would give you an edge in that area, and a rhythm at -100% (at the bottom of that cycle) would make life more difficult in that area. There is no particular meaning to a day on which your rhythms are all high or all low, except the obvious benefits or hindrances that these rare extremes are thought to have on your life.

In addition to the three popular cycles, various other cycles have been proposed, based on linear combination of the three, or on longer or shorter rhythms.[9]

Calculation

Theories published state the equations for the cycles as:

  • physical: ,
  • emotional: ,
  • intellectual: ,

where indicates the number of days since birth. Basic arithmetic shows that the combination of the simpler 23- and 28-day cycles repeats every 644 days (or 1-3/4 years), while the triple combination of 23-, 28-, and 33-day cycles repeats every 21,252 days (or 58.18+ years).

History

The notion of periodic cycles in human fortunes is ancient; for instance, it is found in natal astrology and in folk beliefs about "lucky days". The 23- and 28-day rhythms used by biorhythmists, however, were first devised in the late 19th century by Wilhelm Fliess, a Berlin physician and patient of Sigmund Freud. Fliess believed that he observed regularities at 23- and 28-day intervals in a number of phenomena, including births and deaths. He labeled the 23-day rhythm "male" and the 28-day rhythm "female", matching the menstrual cycle.

In 1904, Viennese psychology professor Hermann Swoboda came to similar conclusions. Alfred Teltscher, professor of engineering at the University of Innsbruck, developed Swoboda's work and suggested that his students' good and bad days followed a rhythmic pattern; he believed that the brain's ability to absorb, mental ability, and alertness ran in 33-day cycles.[9] One of the first academic researchers of biorhythms was Estonian-born Nikolai Pärna, who published a book in German called Rhythm, Life and Creation in 1923.

The practice of consulting biorhythms was popularized in the 1970s by a series of books by Bernard Gittelson, including Biorhythm — A Personal Science, Biorhythm Charts of the Famous and Infamous, and Biorhythm Sports Forecasting. Gittelson's company, Biorhythm Computers, Inc., made a business selling personal biorhythm charts and calculators, but his ability to predict sporting events was not substantiated.[10]

Charting biorhythms for personal use was popular in the United States during the 1970s; many places (especially video arcades and amusement areas) had a biorhythm machine that provided charts upon entry of date of birth. Biorhythm programs were a common application on personal computers; and in the late 1970s, there were also handheld biorhythm calculators on the market, the Kosmos 1 and the Casio Biolator.[11][12] Biorhythm charts appeared in the Chicago Tribune from 1977 to 1979, and Gittelson wrote daily biorhythm charts for the Toronto Star from 1981 to 1985.[13]

Although biorhythms have declined in popularity (pop culture magazine Vice considered them "dead" by the mid 2010s[13]), there are free and proprietary apps and computer programs which have charting and analysis capabilities, as well as numerous websites that offer free biorhythm readings.

Critical views

There have been some three dozen studies supporting biorhythm theory, but according to a study by Terence Hines, all of those had methodological and statistical errors.[14] Hines rejected 134 biorhythm studies and concluded that the theory is not valid.[14]

Supporters continued to defend the theory after Hines' review, causing other scientists to consider the field as pseudoscience:

{{quote|text=An examination of some 134 biorhythm studies found that the theory is not valid (Hines, 1998). It is empirically testable and has been shown to be false. Terence Hines believes that this fact implies that biorhythm theory 'can not be properly termed a pseudoscientific theory'. However, when the advocates of an empirically testable theory refuse to give up the theory in the face of overwhelming evidence against it, it seems reasonable to call the theory pseudoscientific. For, in fact, the adherents to such a theory have declared by their behaviour that there is nothing that could falsify it, yet they continue to claim the theory is scientific. (from Carroll's "The Skeptic's Dictionary")[6]{{rp|175}}}}

The physiologist Gordon Stein in the book Encyclopedia of Hoaxes (1993) has written: "Both the theoretical underpinning and the practical scientific verification of biorhythm theory are lacking. Without those, biorhythms became just another pseudoscientific claim that people are willing to accept without required evidence. Those pushing biorhythm calculators and books on a gullible public are guilty of making fraudulent claims. They are hoaxers of the public if they know what they are saying has no factual justification."[15]

A 1978 study of the incidence of industrial accidents found neither empirical nor theoretical support for the biorhythm model.[16]

Additional Studies Supporting the Classification of Biorhythms as Pseudoscientific

Several controlled, experimental studies have been conducted in order to investigate the validity of biorhythms.[17][18][19] Such studies have yielded results that provide support for the classification of biorhythms as a pseudoscience. These studies include:

Prytula and Colleagues (1980)[17]

They found that subjects did not perceive biorhythms generated from random dates as being any more or less valid than those constructed using their birthdays.[17] Further, these subjects did not display any differences in rating between the 'valid' (i.e., constructed using the subject's birthday), and random biorhythms. Overall, the results of this experiment provide data supporting the argument against the perceived validity of biorhythms.[17]

James (1984)[18]

James hypothesized that if biorhythms were rooted in science, then each proposed biorhythm cycle would contribute to task performance.[18] Further, he predicted that each type of biorhythm cycle (i.e., intellectual, physical, and emotional) would be most influential on tasks associated with the corresponding cycle type. For example, he postulated that intellectual biorhythm cycles would be most influential on academic testing performance.[18] In order to test his hypotheses, James observed 368 participants, noting their performance on tasks associated with intellectual, physical, and emotional functioning. Based on data collected from his experimental research, James concluded the there was no relation between subjects' biorhythmic status (on any of the three cycle types), and their performance on the associated practical tests.[18]

Peveto (1980)[19]

Peveto examined the proposed relationship between biorhythms and academic performance, specifically in terms of reading ability.[19] Through examination of the data collected, Peveto concluded that there were no significant differences in the academic performance of the students, in regards to reading, during the high, low, or critical positions of neither the physical biorhythm cycle, the emotional biorhythm cycle, nor the intellectual biorhythm cycle. As a result, it was concluded that biorhythm cycles have no affect on the academic performance of students, when academic performance was measured using reading ability.[19]

See also

{{Commons category|Biorhythm}}
  • List of topics characterized as pseudoscience
  • Astrology
  • Bioelectricity
  • Chronotherapy (treatment scheduling)
  • Circadian rhythm
  • Fortune-telling

References

1. ^βίος, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus
2. ^ῥυθμός, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus
3. ^{{cite journal |title=Effects of circadian rhythm phase alteration on physiological and psychological variables: Implications to pilot performance (including a partially annotated bibliography) |url=https://ntrs.nasa.gov/search.jsp?R=19810016185&hterms=NASA-TM-81277&qs=Ntx%3Dmode%2520matchallany%26Ntk%3DAll%26Ns%3DLoaded-Date%7C1%26N%3D0%26Ntt%3DNASA-TM-81277 |journal=Nasa-Tm-81277 |date=1981-03-01 |accessdate=2011-05-25|last1=Holley |first1=D. C. Winger }} "No evidence exists to support the concept of biorhythms; in fact, scientific data refute their existence."
4. ^{{Cite book |series=SUNY series in philosophy |chapter=Winning through pseudoscience |author=Clark Glymour, Douglas Stalker |editor=Patrick Grim |edition=2, revised |title=? Philosophy of science and the occult |publisher=SUNY Press |year=1990 |isbn=978-0-7914-0204-7 |pages=92, 94 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=5VewAkDw8h0C |quote=They'll cheerfully empty their pockets to anyone with a twinkle in their eye and a pseudoscience in their pocket. Astrology, biorhythms, ESP, numerology, astral projection, scientology, UFOlogy, pyramid power, psychic surgeons, Atlantis real state (...). (...) your pseudoscience will have better sales potential if it makes use of a mysterious device, or a lot of calculations (but simple calculations) (...) The great models [of this sales potential] are astrology and biorhythms (...)}}.
5. ^{{Cite book |chapter=Science, Protoscience and Pseudoscience |author=Raimo Toumela |title=Rational changes in science: essays on scientific reasoning |series=Boston studies in the philosophy of science |volume=98 |editor=Joseph C. Pitt, Marcello Pera |edition=illustrated |publisher=Springer |year=1987 |isbn=978-90-277-2417-5 |pages=94, 96 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=9e_-_O1OgTYC |quote=If we take such pseudosciences as astrology, the theory of biorhythms, suitable parts of parapsychology, homeopathy and faith healing (...) Such examples of pseudoscience as the theory of biorhythms, astrology, dianetics, creationism, [and] faith healing may seem too obvious examples of pseudoscience for academic readers.}}
6. ^{{Cite book |chapter=Metatheoretical problems in phonology with Occam's Razor and non-ad-hoc-ness |author= Stefan Ploch |title=Living on the edge: 28 papers in honour of Jonathan Kaye |series=Studies in generative grammar |editor=Jonathan Kaye, Stefan Ploch |publisher=Walter de Gruyter |year=2003 |isbn=978-3-11-017619-3 |pages=166, 174–176, 186, footnotes 15 and 17 in page 199 |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=0WT96J7swIwC |quote=the following quote about the pseudoscientific biorhythm theory [p. 174–175] (...) we can eliminate ad hoc hypotheses (i.e. arbitrariness) that are the hallmark of all pseudosciences (astrology, biorhythm theory, (...) [p. 176] Unfortunately, in the case of the most socially successful [not scientific] theories, just as in the case of astrology and biorhythm "theory", we are dealing with something that resembles quackery closely. [p.186] (...) what matters is that falsifying data is systematically discounted in this pseudotheory. [p. 199]}}.
7. ^{{Cite web|url=https://biorhythm.xyz/?dob=1990-01-29|title=Biorhythm chart|last=|first=|date=|website=Biorhythm . XYZ|archive-url=|archive-date=|dead-url=|access-date=2018-10-29}}
8. ^These cycles are to be adjusted based on the person's personal day clock which may run from 22 hours to 27 hours although 23-25 is the norm. Two ways you can find your personal day clock is grip test and body temperature every 15 minutes for a few days or easier same time each day for a few months.
9. ^http://www.skepdic.com/biorhyth.html
10. ^Hoffmann, Frank W., and William G. Bailey, Mind and Society Fads, 1992.
11. ^http://www.vintage-technology.info/pages/calculators/k/kosmos1.htm
12. ^{{Cite web | url=http://mycalcdb.free.fr/main.php?l=0&id=326 | title=MyCalcDB : Calculator Casio BIOLATOR aka H-801}}
13. ^{{cite news|last1=Heisel|first1=Andrew|title=Biorhythms: The 1970s Fad That Won a Super Bowl, Killed Clark Gable, and Made America Gaga for Computers {{!}} VICE Sports|url=https://sports.vice.com/en_us/article/biorhythms-the-1970s-fad-that-won-a-super-bowl-killed-clark-gable-and-made-america-gaga-for-computers|accessdate=27 December 2016|work=VICE Sports|date=15 September 2015|language=en}}
14. ^{{cite journal|title=Comprehensive Review of Biorhythm Theory |journal=Psychological Reports |year=1998 |first=Terence |last=Hines |volume=83 |pages=19–64 |doi=10.2466/PR0.83.5.19-64 |url=http://ammonsscientific.com/link.php?N=10326 |format=PDF (summary) |accessdate=2008-01-13 |pmid=9775660 |issue=1 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20090212181753/http://ammonsscientific.com/link.php?N=10326 |archivedate=February 12, 2009 }}
15. ^Stein, Gordon. (1993). Encyclopedia of Hoaxes. Gale Group. p. 161. {{ISBN|0-8103-8414-0}}
16. ^{{cite journal | pmid = 662540 | doi=10.2466/pms.1978.46.2.423 | volume=46 | issue=2 | title=No evidence for relationship between biorhythms and industrial accidents | journal=Percept mot Skills | pages=423–6 | last1 = Persinger | first1 = MA | last2 = Cooke | first2 = WJ | last3 = Janes | first3 = JT| year=1978 }}
17. ^{{Cite journal |last=Prytula |first=Robert E |display-authors=et al |date=1980 |title=Studies on the perceived predictive accuracy of biorhythms |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=65 |issue=6 |pages=723–727 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.65.6.723 |issn=0021-9010}}
18. ^{{Cite journal |last=James |first=Arthur |date=1984 |title=The validity of 'biorhythmic' theory questioned |journal=British Journal of Psychology |volume=75 |issue=2 |pages=197–200 |doi=10.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01891.x |issn=0007-1269}}
19. ^{{Cite journal|last=Peveto|first=Natalia|date=1980-01-01|title=The Relationship of Biorhythms to Academic Performance in Reading.|url=https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/3577|journal=LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses}}

Further reading

Books

  • Aschoff, Jurgen (ed.), Biological Rhythms (Handbooks of Behavioral Neurobiology). 1981.
  • Bartel, Pauline C., "Biorhythm: discovering your natural ups and downs", An Impact book. {{ISBN|0-531-01355-3}}
  • Bentley, Evie, Awareness: biorhythms, sleep, and dreaming. {{ISBN|0-415-18872-5}}
  • Crawley, Jacyntha, The Biorhythm Kit, UK: {{ISBN|1-85906-032-3}}, London Biorhythm Company Limited.
  • Edlund, Matthew. Psychological time and mental illness. 1987. {{ISBN|0-89876-122-0}}
  • Evans, James R., (ed.) and Manfred Clynes (ed.), Rhythm in psychological, linguistic, and musical processes. {{ISBN|0-398-05235-2}}
  • Gittelson, Bernard, Biorhythm: A Personal Science, Futura Publications. 1976 {{ISBN|0-86007-361-0}}
  • Hodgkins, Zerrin Biomatch Z. 1998. {{ISBN|0-9531983-0-8}}
  • Lapointe, Fernand, Biorythmie: comment prâevoir vos bons et mauvais jours. {{ISBN|0-88566-029-3}}
  • Louis, Arthur M., Journalism and Other Atrocities. 2010. {{ISBN|978-1-4538-1520-5}}
  • Roche, James, Biorhythms at your fingertips. {{ISBN|0-7137-1562-6}}
  • Thommen, George S., Is This Your Day. 1973. {{ISBN|0-517-00742-8}}
  • Debarbieux, Patrick, l'ABC des biorythmes. 1999. {{ISBN|2-7339-0615-1}}

Research publications

  • Hines, T.M., "Comprehensive review of biorhythm theory". Psychology Department, Pace University, Pleasantville, NY. Psychol Rep. 1998 Aug;83(1):19–64. (ed. concluded that biorhythm theory is not valid.)
  • D'Andrea, V.J., D.R. Black, and N.G. Stayrook, "Relation of the Fliess-Swoboda Biorhythm Theory to suicide occurrence". J Nerv Ment Dis. 1984 Aug;172(8):490–4. (ed. concluded that there was a validity to biorhythm when the innovative methods of the study are put to use.)
  • Laxenaire M., and O. Laurent, "What is the current thinking on the biorhythm theory?" Ann Med Psychol (Paris). 1983 Apr;141(4):425–9. [French] (ed. Biorhythm theory is disregarded by the medical world though it has achieved a bit of fame with the public)
  • {{cite journal | last1 = Wolcott | first1 = J.H. | last2 = McMeekin | first2 = R.R. | last3 = Burgin | first3 = R.E. | last4 = Yanowitch | first4 = R.E. | date = Jun 1977 | title = Correlation of general aviation accidents with the biorhythm theory | url = | journal = Hum Factors | volume = 19 | issue = 3| pages = 283–93 | pmid = 873528 | doi = 10.1177/001872087701900311 }}
  • {{cite journal | last1 = Khalil | first1 = T.M. | last2 = Kurucz | first2 = C.N. | date = Jul 1977 | title = The influence of 'biorhythm' on accident occurrence and performance | url = | journal = Ergonomics | volume = 20 | issue = 4| pages = 389–98 | doi=10.1080/00140137708931641| pmid = 908322 }}
  • "Biorhythm in gynecology--a renaissance of Fliess' theory of periodicity?". Arch Gynecol. 1979 20 July;228(1-4):642. [German]
  • Nijsten, M.W., and S.E. Willemsen, "Accidents a matter of chance? The significance of lunar phases and biorhythms in trauma patients". Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd. 1991 21 December;135(51):2421–4. [Dutch] (ed. 'critical' biorhythm days were not found to increase the number of accidents experienced by subjects.)
{{Pseudoscience}}{{Authority control}}

4 : Articles with inconsistent citation formats|Sleep|Pseudoscience|Waveforms

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/11 7:34:00