词条 | Contact hypothesis |
释义 |
In psychology and other social sciences, the contact hypothesis suggests that intergroup contact under appropriate conditions can effectively reduce prejudice between majority and minority group members. Following WWII and the desegregation of the military and other public institutions, policymakers and social scientists had turned an eye towards the policy implications of interracial contact. Of them, social psychologist Gordon Allport united early research in this vein under intergroup contact theory. In 1954, Allport published The Nature of Prejudice, in which he outlined the most widely cited form of the hypothesis.[1] The premise of Allport's hypothesis states that under appropriate conditions interpersonal contact could be one of the most effective ways to reduce prejudice between majority and minority group members.[2] According to Allport, properly managed contact should reduce issues of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination that commonly occur between rival groups and lead to better intergroup interactions. In the decades following Allport's book, social scientists expanded and applied the contact hypothesis towards the reduction of prejudice beyond racism, including prejudice towards physically and mentally disabled people, women, and LGBT people, in hundreds of different studies.[3] In some subfields of criminology, psychology, and sociology, intergroup contact has been described as one of the best ways to improve relations among groups in conflict.[4][5] Nonetheless, the effects of intergroup contact vary widely from context to context, and empirical inquiry continues to this day.[6] HistoryWhile Gordon W. Allport is often credited with the development of the contact hypothesis, the idea that interpersonal contact could improve intergroup relations was not a novel one. In the 1930s and 1940s, writers had already begun speculating about the outcomes of interracial contact.[7][8][9] In 1947, sociologist R. M. Williams described interpersonal collaboration with goal interdependence as a worthwhile strategy to reduce intergroup hostility.[10] Following WWII, social scientists examined the effects of desegregation on racial attitudes in the U.S. Merchant Marine, and in desegregated New York City housing projects.[11][12] In 1951, as national attention turned to issues of desegregation in schools, ultimately leading up to Brown v. Board of Education, Robert Carter and Thurgood Marshall, from the NAACP Legal Defense and Educational Fund, solicited expert opinions from social science.[13][14] A range of social scientists, from Kenneth Clark to Floyd and Gordon Allport, weighed in on the psychological effects of desegregation, and conditions under which interracial contact might attenuate racial prejudice, including an amicus curiae brief filed in the Brown v. Board case.[15] Other studies have claimed that contact hypothesis is a very simple and optimistic and that contact would most likely gravitate toward hostility rather than friendship if two competitive parties were involved. If groups with a negative outlook were brought together, it would lead to increases of negative attitudes rather than positive.[16] Allport situated his formulation of the contact hypothesis in broader discussion of racial diversity—a precursor to interracial proximity and contact. While diversity more generally might foment conflict and prejudice, Allport suggested that contact, under four particular conditions, would facilitate intergroup understanding and consequently reduce prejudice. Conditions of intergroup contactIn the years prior to Allport’s framing of intergroup contact theory, social scientists had already begun discussing the conditions of intergroup contact that would produce intergroup anxiety, prejudice, or other “detrimental psychological effects”.[17] Wilner, Walkley, & Cook, two years prior to The Nature of Prejudice, studied segregation and integration in housing projects, and also suggested four conditions under which intergroup attitudes would change for the better. Under the assumption that prejudice arises from racial segregation, they suggested that it would diminish when members occupy “the same or equivalent roles in the situation,” share background characteristics like education, age, or socioeconomic status, perceive common interests or goals, and when the “social climate […] is not unfavorable to interracial association.”[18] Concurrently, Carolyn Sherif and Muzafer Sherif developed their Robbers Cave experiment, an illustration of realistic conflict theory.[19] The Sherifs highlighted the importance of superordinate goals and equal status between groups, but notably, did not weigh in alongside other social scientists in their amicus brief for Brown v. Board of Education. In Allport's own words, {{Quote |text="[Prejudice] may be reduced by equal status contact between majority and minority groups in the pursuit of common goals. The effect is greatly enhanced if this contact is sanctioned by institutional supports (i.e., by law, custom, or local atmosphere), and provided it is of a sort that leads to the perception of common interests and common humanity between members of the two groups." |author=Gordon W. Allport|source=The Nature of Prejudice (1954) }} In other words, four conditions under which intergroup contact will reduce prejudice are:
Additionally, Allport specified that within intergroup cooperation, personal interaction, involving informal, personal interaction between group members would scaffold learning about each other and the formation of cross-group friendships. Yet, without these conditions, casual, or superficial, contact would cause people to resort to stereotypes.[1] The largest meta-analysis of the contact literature suggested that the conditions are facilitating but not essential.[20] However, more recent meta-analysis highlights that many configurations of the conditions have not yet been experimentally tested.[6] Psychological processes involved in intergroup contactA number of psychological processes have been hypothesised to explain how and why intergroup contact is able to reduce prejudice and improve intergroup relations. Firstly, Allport (1954) argued that intergroup contact facilitates learning about the outgroup, and this new outgroup knowledge leads to prejudice reduction.[1] Secondly, intergroup contact is believed to reduce the fear and anxiety people have when interacting with the outgroup, which in turn reduces their negative evaluations of the outgroup.[21] Thirdly, intergroup contact is hypothesised to increase people's ability to take the perspective of the outgroup and empathize with their concerns.[22] Empirical research has only found weak support for role of outgroup knowledge in prejudice reduction; however, the affective mechanisms of intergroup anxiety and outgroup empathy have accumulated extensive empirical support.[23] The reduction of prejudice through intergroup contact can be described as the reconceptualization of group categories. Allport (1954) claimed that prejudice is a direct result of generalizations and oversimplifications made about an entire group of people based on incomplete or mistaken information. The basic rationale is that prejudice may be reduced as one learns more about a category of people.[1] Rothbart and John (1985) describe belief change through contact as "an example of the general cognitive process by which attributes of category members modify category attributes" (p. 82).[24] An individual's beliefs can be modified by that person coming into contact with a culturally distinct category member and subsequently modifying or elaborating the beliefs about the category as a whole. However, contact fails to cure conflict when contact situations create anxiety for those who take part. Contact situations need to be long enough to allow this anxiety to decrease and for the members of the conflicting groups to feel comfortable with one another.{{Citation needed|date=November 2018}} Additionally if the members of the two groups use this contact situation to trade insults, argue with each other, resort to physical violence, and discriminate against each other, then contact should not be expected to reduce conflict between groups. To obtain beneficial effects, the situation must include positive contact. The effects of intergroup contactSocial scientists have documented positive effects of intergroup contact across field, experimental, and correlational studies, across a variety of contact situations, and between various social groups. Pettigrew and Tropp's canonical 2006 meta-analysis of 515 separate studies found general support for the contact hypothesis.[25] Furthermore, their analysis found that face-to-face contact between group members significantly reduced prejudice; the more contact groups had, the less prejudice group members reported.[26] Moreover, the beneficial effects of intergroup contact were significantly greater when the contact situation was structured to include Allport's facilitating conditions for optimal contact. ExamplesIntergroup contact and prejudice towards African AmericansThe majority of intergroup contact research has focused on reducing prejudice towards African Americans. For example, in one study, Brown, Brown, Jackson, Sellers, and Manuel (2003) investigated the amount of contact white athletes had with black teammates and whether the athletes played an individual or team sport. Team sports (e.g., football or basketball), as opposed to individual sports (e.g., track or swimming), require teamwork and cooperative interactions to win. Results showed that White athletes who played team sports reported less prejudice than athletes who played individual sports.[27] Intergroup contact and prejudice towards homosexualityThe contact hypothesis has proven to be highly effective in alleviating prejudice directed toward homosexuals.[28] Applying the contact hypothesis to heterosexuals and homosexuals, Herek (1987) found that college students who had pleasant interactions with a homosexual tend to generalize from that experience and accept homosexuals as a group.[29] Herek and Glunt's (1993) national study of interpersonal contact and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men found that increased contact "predicted attitudes toward gay men better than did any other demographic or social psychological variable" (p. 239); such variables included gender, race, age, education, geographic residence, marital status, number of children, religion and political ideology.[30] Herek and Capitanio (1996) found that contact experiences with two or three homosexuals are associated with more favorable attitudes than are contact experiences with only one individual.[31] Intergroup contact and prejudice towards Muslims in EuropeSavelkoul et al. (2011) in their study from the Netherlands found people living in regions with high numbers of Muslims (i.e. those more exposed to unavoidable intergroup contacts) get used to and are more experienced with their integration and express lesser perceived threats. In addition, they also found that higher contacts with Muslim colleagues directly reduce Islamophobia\\anti-Muslim attitudes.[32] Similarly, Novotny and Polonsky (2011) in their survey among Czech and Slovak university students documented that personal contacts with Muslims and experience with visiting an Islamic country associate with more positive attitudes towards Muslims.[33] However Agirdag et al. (2012) report that Belgian teachers working in schools that enroll a larger share of Muslim students have more negative attitudes toward Muslim students than other teachers.[34] Intergroup contact with immigrant groups within the United StatesDaniel J. Hopkins presented the idea that local conditions within a community or in other words changes in local immigrant demographics can affect the attitudes of people on immigrants. This can include the stereotyping of immigrants and/or a development of pro or anti-immigrant ideas. These attitudes may be shaped by experiences the non-immigrant population has with the immigrant population. He believes that this idea is not necessarily universal but that certain conditions play a role on the development of attitudes[35]. Indirect intergroup contactOne of the most important advances in research on intergroup contact is the growing evidence for a number of indirect, non-face-to-face intergroup contact strategies as a means to improve relations between social groups.[36] While the benefits of direct intergroup contact have been empirically established, its implementation is often not practical. For example, in many countries, racial and religious groups are often residentially, educationally or occupationally segregated, which limits the opportunity for direct contact. However, even when the opportunity for direct intergroup contact is high, anxiety and fear can produce a negative or hostile contact experience or lead to the avoidance of the contact situation altogether. Indirect forms of intergroup contact include: Extended contactThe extended contact hypothesis, established by Wright and colleagues in 1997, posits that knowing that a member of one's own group has a close relationship with a member of an outgroup can lead to more positive attitudes towards that outgroup. Correlational research has demonstrated that individuals who report knowledge that an ingroup member has an outgroup friend typically report more positive outgroup attitudes, while experimental research has shown that providing ingroup members with this information creates the same positive effect.[37] In the 20 years since its proposal, the extended contact hypothesis has guided over 100 studies, that generally find support for the positive effect of extended contact on prejudice reduction, independent of direct friendship with outgroup members.[38][39] In a similar vein, vicarious contact involves simply observing an ingroup member interact with an outgroup member.[40] For example, positive media portrayals of intergroup interactions on television and radio (also known as parasocial contact) have the potential to reduce the prejudice of millions of viewers and listeners.[41] Imagined contact{{Main|Imagined contact hypothesis}}The 'imagined contact hypothesis' was put forward by Richard J. Crisp and Rhiannon Turner (2009)[42] and proposes that simply imagining a positive encounter with a member or members of an outgroup category can promote more positive intergroup attitudes. Electronic- or E-contactWhite and her colleagues (2012; 2014) recently developed Electronic- or E-contact. E-contact involves an ingroup member interacting with an outgroup member over the Internet[43][44] and includes text-based, video-based or a mixture of both text- and video-based online interactions. Electronic contact has been empirically shown to reduce inter-religious prejudice between Christian and Muslim students in Australia in both the short[45] and long term[46], as well between Catholic and Protestant students in Northern Ireland[47]. In the context of sexual prejudice, research also has shown that interacting online with a member of the outgroup is a particularly useful particularly useful as a prejudice-reduction strategy among individuals who typically report ideologically intolerant beliefs[48]. Additionally, in the context of mental health stigma, participants who experienced a brief interaction with a person diagnosed with schizophrenia reported reduced fear, anger, and stereotyping toward people with schizophrenia in general compared to a control condition[49]. CriticismsWhile large bodies of research have been devoted to examining intergroup contact, social scientific reviews of the literature frequently voice skepticism about the likelihood of contact's optimal conditions occurring in concert, and by extension, about the generalizability of correlational research and lab studies on contact.[50] Null findings and gaps in researchThough the general findings of intergroup contact research have inspired promise, Bertrand and Duflo (2017) find that observational correlations between intergroup contact and non-prejudiced behavior can be explained by self-selection: less prejudiced people seek out contact.[51] Comparatively, fewer controlled experimental studies of intergroup contact exist; of those that do, few measure prejudice outcomes longer than one day after treatment, leaving a gap in the literature that investigates the long-term effects of contact. Furthermore, of these experiments, none measure the reduction of racial prejudice in people over the age of 25.[6] Similarly, in a report to the United Kingdom Equality and Human Rights Commission, Dominic Abrams highlights "a dearth of good-quality longitudinal research on prejudice or prejudice reduction”.[52] Gordon Allport himself suggested that in light of increasing racial contact in the United States, "the more contact the more trouble", unless scaffolded by the four facilitating conditions he proposed, distinguishing casual contact and "true acquaintance" or "knowledge-giving contact".[1] In political science, Allport's work is often juxtaposed with V.O. Key's examination of Southern politics, which found that racism grew in areas where the local concentrations of black Americans were higher.[53] In that context, absent the specific conditions of Allport, contact comes to produce more negative effects, namely increasing prejudice. Some social psychologists have converged with political scientists on this position.[54] Negative contactPaolini, Harwood, and Rubin (2010) proposed that intergroup contact may have more negative than positive effects on prejudice, because it makes outgroup members' social group more salient during encounters,[55] the [https://sites.google.com/site/markrubinsocialpsychresearch/positive-and-negative-experiences-with-members-of-other-groups negative contact hypothesis]. Recent evidence suggests that although negative intergroup contact is more influential than positive intergroup contact, it is also less common than positive contact in real world intergroup encounters, in five central European countries.[56][57] Recent research also suggests that people's past experiences with out-group members moderates contact: people who have had positive experiences with out-group members in the past show a smaller discrepancy between the effects of positive and negative contact.[58] See also{{portal|Psychology}}{{col div|colwidth=30em}}
References1. ^1 2 3 4 Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books 2. ^Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books 3. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Pettigrew | first1 = T. F. | last2 = Tropp | first2 = L. R. | year = 2006 | title = A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 90 | issue = 5| pages = 751–783 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 | pmid=16737372}} 4. ^Brown, R., & Hewstone, M. (2005). An integrative theory of intergroup contact. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 37,pp. 255–343). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. 5. ^Wright, S. C. (2009). Cross-group contact effects. In S. Otten, T. Kessler & K. Sassenberg (Eds.), Intergroup relations: The role of emotion and motivation (pp. 262–283). New York, NY: Psychology Press. 6. ^1 2 {{cite journal |last1=Paluck |first1=Elizabeth Levy |last2=Green |first2=Seth Ariel |last3=Green |first3=Donald |title=The contact hypothesis re-evaluated |journal=Behavioural Public Policy |date=2018 |pages=1–30}} 7. ^{{cite book |last1=Baker |first1=Paul Earnest |title=Negro–White adjustment |date=1934 |publisher=Association Press |location=New York |page=120}} 8. ^{{cite journal |last1=Lett |first1=H |title=Techniques for achieving interracial cooperation |journal=Proceedings of the Institute on Race Relations and Community Organization. Chicago: University of Chicago and the American Council on Race Relations |date=1945}} 9. ^{{cite book |last1=Brameld |first1=T |title=Minority problems in the public schools |date=1946 |publisher=Harper |location=New York}} 10. ^{{cite book |last1=Williams |first1=R. M. |title=The reduction of intergroup tensions |date=1947 |publisher=Social Science Research Council |location=New York}} 11. ^{{cite journal |last1=Brophy |first1=I. N. |title=The luxury of anti-Negro prejudice |journal=Public Opinion Quarterly |date=1946 |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=456–466|doi=10.1086/265762 }} 12. ^{{cite book |last1=Deutsch |first1=M |last2=Collins|first2=M |title=Interracial housing: A psychological evaluation of a social experiment |date=1951 |publisher=University of Minnesota Press |location=Minneapolis}} 13. ^{{cite book |last1=Kluger |first1=Richard |title=Simple Justice: The History of Brown v. Board of Education and Black America's Struggle for Equality |date=2004 |publisher=Vintage Books |isbn=9781400030613}} 14. ^{{cite book |last1=Wrightsman |first1=Lawrence |title=Oral Arguments Before the Supreme Court: An Empirical Approach |date=2008 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0195368628}} 15. ^{{cite journal |last1=Allport |first1=Floyd |title=The effects of segregation and the consequences of desegregation: A social science statement |journal=Journal of Negro Education |date=1953 |volume=22 |issue=1 |pages=68–76|doi=10.2307/2293629 |jstor=2293629 }} 16. ^Amir, Y. (1976). The role of intergroup contact in the change of prejudice and ethnic relations. In P.A. Katz (Ed.), Towards the elimination of racism (pp. 245-308).New York: Pergamon. 17. ^{{cite journal |last1=Chein |first1=Isidor |title=What are the psychological effects of segregation under conditions of equal facilities?What are the psychological effects of segregation under conditions of equal facilities? |journal=What Are the Psychological Effects of Segregation Under Conditions of Equal Facilities? |date=1949 |volume=3 |pages=229–234}} 18. ^{{cite journal |last1=Wilner |first1=Daniel M. |last2=Walkley |first2=Rosabelle Price |last3=Cook |first3=Stuart W. |title=Residential Proximity and Intergroup Relations in Public Housing Projects |journal=Journal of Social Issues |date=1952 |pages=45–69}} 19. ^{{cite book|author=Sherif, M.|author2=Harvey, O.J.|author3=White, B.J.|author4=Hood, W.|author5=Sherif, C.W.|lastauthoramp=y|title=Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment|year=1961|publisher=The University Book Exchange|location=Norman, OK|pages=155–184}} 20. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Pettigrew | first1 = T. F. | last2 = Tropp | first2 = L. R. | year = 2006 | title = A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 90 | issue = 5| pages = 751–783 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 | pmid=16737372}} 21. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Stephan | first1 = W. G. | last2 = Stephan | first2 = C. W. | year = 1985 | title = Intergroup anxiety | url = | journal = Journal of Social Issues | volume = 41 | issue = 3| pages = 157–175 | doi=10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01134.x}} 22. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Stephan | first1 = W. G. | last2 = Finlay | first2 = K. | year = 1999 | title = The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations | url = | journal = Journal of Social Issues | volume = 55 | issue = 4| pages = 729–743 | doi=10.1111/0022-4537.00144}} 23. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Pettigrew | first1 = T. F. | last2 = Tropp | first2 = L. R. | year = 2008 | title = How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta‐analytic tests of three mediators | url = | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 38 | issue = 6| pages = 922–934 | doi=10.1002/ejsp.504}} 24. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Rothbart | first1 = M. | last2 = John | first2 = O. P. | year = 1985 | title = Social categorization and behavioral episodes: A cognitive analysis of the effects of intergroup contact | url = | journal = Journal of Social Issues | volume = 41 | issue = 3| pages = 81–104 | doi=10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01130.x}} 25. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Pettigrew | first1 = T. F. | last2 = Tropp | first2 = L. R. | year = 2006 | title = A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 90 | issue = 5| pages = 751–783 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 | pmid=16737372}} 26. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Pettigrew | first1 = T. F. | last2 = Tropp | first2 = L. R. | year = 2006 | title = A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 90 | issue = 5| pages = 751–783 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 | pmid=16737372}} 27. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Brown | first1 = K.T | last2 = Brown | first2 = T.N. | last3 = Jackson | first3 = J.S. | last4 = Sellers | first4 = R.M. | last5 = Manuel | first5 = W.J. | year = 2003 | title = Teammates on and off the field? Contact with Black teammates and the racial attitudes of White student athletes | url = | journal = Journal of Applied Social Psychology | volume = 33 | issue = 7| pages = 1379–1403 | doi=10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01954.x}} 28. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Smith | first1 = S. J. | last2 = Axelton | first2 = A. M. | last3 = Saucier | first3 = D. A. | year = 2009 | title = The effects of contact on sexual prejudice: A meta-analysis | url = | journal = Sex Roles | volume = 61 | issue = 3–4| pages = 178–191 | doi=10.1007/s11199-009-9627-3}} 29. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Herek | first1 = G. M. | year = 1987 | title = The instrumentality of attitudes: Toward a neofunctional theory | url = | journal = Journal of Social Issues | volume = 42 | issue = 2| pages = 99–114 | doi=10.1111/j.1540-4560.1986.tb00227.x}} 30. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Herek | first1 = G. M. | last2 = Glunt | first2 = E. K. | year = 1993 | title = Interpersonal contact and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men: Results from a national survey | url = | journal = Journal of Sex Research | volume = 30 | issue = 3| pages = 239–244 | doi=10.1080/00224499309551707}} 31. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Herek | first1 = G. M. | last2 = Capitanio | first2 = J. P. | year = 1996 | title = Some of my best friends": Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men and lesbians Personality | url = | journal = Social Psychology Bulletin | volume = 22 | issue = 4| pages = 412–424 | doi=10.1177/0146167296224007}} 32. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Savelkoul | first1 = Scheepers | last2 = Tolsma | first2 = J. | last3 = Hagendoorn | first3 = L. | year = 2011 | title = Anti-Muslim Attitudes in The Netherlands: Tests of Contradictory Hypotheses Derived from Ethnic Competition Theory and Intergroup Contact Theory | url = | journal = European Sociological Review | volume = 27 | issue = 6| pages = 741–758 | doi=10.1093/esr/jcq035| hdl = 2066/99505 }} 33. ^Novotny, J., Polonsky, F. (2011): The Level of Knowledge about Islam and Perception of Islam among Czech and Slovak University Students: does Ignorance Determine Subjective Attitudes? Sociologia, 43, 6, 674-696. 34. ^{{cite journal|last1=Agirdag|first1=Orhan|last2=Loobuyck|first2=Patrick|last3=Van Houtte|first3=Mieke|title=Determinants of Attitudes Toward Muslim Students Among Flemish Teachers: A Research Note|journal=Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion|volume=51|issue=2|pages=368–376|doi=10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01637.x|year=2012}} 35. ^{{cite journal|last1=Hopkins|first1=Daniel J.|title=Politicized Places: Explaining Where and When Immigrants Provoke Local Opposition|journal=American Political Science Review|date=2010|volume=104|issue=1|pages=40–60|doi=10.1017/S0003055409990360|language=en|issn=1537-5943|citeseerx=10.1.1.500.3856}} 36. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Dovidio | first1 = J. F. | last2 = Eller | first2 = A. | last3 = Hewstone | first3 = M. | year = 2011 | title = Improving intergroup relations through direct, extended and other forms of indirect contact | url = | journal = Group Processes & Intergroup Relations | volume = 14 | issue = 2| pages = 147–160 | doi=10.1177/1368430210390555}} 37. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Wright | first1 = S. C. | last2 = Aron | first2 = A. | last3 = McLaughlin-Volpe | first3 = T. | last4 = Ropp | first4 = S. A. | year = 1997 | title = The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice | url = | journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | volume = 73 | issue = | pages = 73–90 | doi=10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.73}} 38. ^{{cite journal |last1=Vezzali |first1=Loris |last2=Hewstone |first2=Miles |last3=Capozza |first3=Dora |last4=Giovanni |first4=Dino |last5=Wolfer |first5=Ralf |title=Improving intergroup relations with extended and vicarious forms of indirect contact |journal=European Review of Social Psychology |date=2014 |volume=25 |pages=314–389 |doi=10.1080/10463283.2014.982948}} 39. ^{{cite journal |last1=Zhou |first1=Shelly |last2=Page-Gould |first2=Elizabeth |last3=Aron |first3=Arthur |last4=Moyer |first4=Anne |last5=Hewstone |first5=Miles |title=The extended contact hypothesis: A meta-analysis on 20 years of research |journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review |date=2018 |pages=1–29}} 40. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Mazziotta | first1 = A. | last2 = Mummendey | first2 = A. | last3 = Wright | first3 = C. S. | year = 2011 | title = Vicarious intergroup contact effects: Applying social-cognitive theory to intergroup contact research | url = | journal = Group Processes & Intergroup Relations | volume = 14 | issue = 2| pages = 255–274 | doi=10.1177/1368430210390533}} 41. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Schiappa | first1 = E. | last2 = Gregg | first2 = P. | last3 = Hewes | first3 = D. | year = 2005 | title = The Parasocial Contact Hypothesis | url = | journal = Communication Monographs | volume = 72 | issue = | pages = 92–115 | doi=10.1080/0363775052000342544}} 42. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Crisp | first1 = R. J. | last2 = Turner | first2 = R. N. | year = 2009 | title = Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions? Reducing prejudice through simulated social contact | url = | journal = American Psychologist | volume = 64 | issue = 4| pages = 231–240 | doi=10.1037/a0014718 | pmid=19449982}} 43. ^{{cite journal | last1 = White | first1 = F. A. | last2 = Abu-Rayya | first2 = H. | last3 = Harvey | first3 = L. J. |year = 2015 | title = Improving intergroup relations in the Internet age: A critical review. | url = | journal = Review of General Psychology | volume = 19 | issue = 2| pages = 129–139 | doi=10.1037/gpr0000036}} 44. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Amichai-Hamburger| first1 = Y. | last2 = McKenna | first2 = K. Y. |year = 2006 | title = The contact hypothesis reconsidered: Interacting via the Internet | url = | journal = Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication | volume = 11 | issue = 3| pages = 825–843 | doi=10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00037.x}} 45. ^{{cite journal | last1 = White | first1 = F. A. | last2 = Abu-Rayya | first2 = H. | year = 2012 | title = A dual identity-electronic contact (DIEC) experiment promoting short- and long-term intergroup harmony | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Social Psychology | volume = 48 | issue = 3| pages = 597–608 | doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2012.01.007}} 46. ^{{cite journal | last1 = White | first1 = F. A. | last2 = Abu-Rayya | first2 = H. | last3 = Weitzel | first3 = C. | year = 2014 | title = Achieving twelve-months of intergroup bias reduction: The dual identity-electronic contact (DIEC) experiment | url = | journal = International Journal of Intercultural Relations | volume = 38 | issue = | pages = 158–163 | doi=10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.08.002}} 47. ^{{cite journal | last1 = White | first1 = F. A. | last2 = Turner | first2 = R. N. | last3 = Verrelli | first3 = S. | last4 = Harvey | first4 = L. J. | last5 = Hanna | first5 = J. R. | year = 2018 | title = Improving intergroup relations between Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland via E‐contact | url = | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = | issue = | pages = | doi=10.1002/ejsp.2515}} 48. ^{{cite journal | last1 = White | first1 = F. A. | last2 = Verrelli | first2 = S. | last3 = Maunder | first3 = R. D. | last4 = Kervinen| first4 = A. | year = 2018 | title = Using electronic contact to reduce homonegative attitudes, emotions, and behavioral intentions among heterosexual women and men: A contemporary extension of the contact hypothesis | url = | journal = The Journal of Sex Research | volume = | issue = | pages = 1–13| doi=10.1080/00224499.2018.1491943| pmid = 30019950 }} 49. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Maunder | first1 = R. D. | last2 = White | first2 = F. A. | last3 = Verrelli | first3 = S. | year = 2018 | title = Modern avenues for intergroup contact: Using E-contact and intergroup emotions to reduce stereotyping and social distancing against people with schizophrenia | url = | journal = Group Processes & Intergroup Relations | volume = | issue = | pages = 136843021879487| doi=10.1177/1368430218794873}} 50. ^{{cite journal |last1=Dixon |first1=John |last2=Durrheim |first2=Kevin |last3=Tredoux |first3=Colin |title=Beyond the optimal contact strategy: A reality check for the contact hypothesis |journal=American Psychologist |date=2005 |volume=60 |issue=7 |pages=697–711|doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.697 |pmid=16221003 }} 51. ^{{cite book |last1=Bertrand |first1=Marianne |last2=Duflo |first2=Esther |title=Field Experiments on Discrimination |journal=Handbook of Economic Field Experiments |date=2017 |volume=1 |pages=309–393|doi=10.1016/bs.hefe.2016.08.004 |isbn=9780444633248 }} 52. ^{{cite journal |last1=Abrams |first1=Dominic |title=Processes of prejudice: Theory, evidence and intervention |journal=Human Rights |date=2010 |page=68}} 53. ^{{cite book |last1=Key |first1=V. O. |title=Southern Politics in State and Nation |date=1949 |publisher=University of Tennessee Press |isbn=9780870494352}} 54. ^{{cite book |last1=Enos |first1=Ryan |title=The Space between Us: Social Geography and Politics |date=2017 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-1108420648}} 55. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Paolini | first1 = S. | last2 = Harwood | first2 = J. | last3 = Rubin | first3 = M. | year = 2010 | title = Negative intergroup contact makes group memberships salient: Explaining why intergroup conflict endures | url = | journal = Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | volume = 36 | issue = 12| pages = 1723–1738 | doi = 10.1177/0146167210388667 | pmid = 21051766 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.1004.4213 }} 56. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Barlow | first1 = F. K. | last2 = Paolini | first2 = S. | last3 = Pedersen | first3 = A. | last4 = Hornsey | first4 = M. J. | last5 = Radke | first5 = H. R. M. | last6 = Harwood | first6 = J. | last7 = Rubin | first7 = M. | last8 = Sibley | first8 = C. G. | year = 2012 | title = The contact caveat: Negative contact predicts increased prejudice more than positive contact predicts reduced prejudice | url = | journal = Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | volume = 38 | issue = 12| pages = 1629–1643 | doi = 10.1177/0146167212457953 | pmid = 22941796 }} 57. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Graf | first1 = S. | last2 = Paolini | first2 = S. | last3 = Rubin | first3 = M. | year = 2014 | title = Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries | url = | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 44 | issue = 6| pages = 536–547 | doi = 10.1002/ejsp.2052 }} 58. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Paolini | first1 = S. | last2 = Harwood | first2 = J. | last3 = Rubin | first3 = M. | last4 = Husnu | first4 = S. | last5 = Joyce | first5 = N. | last6 = Hewstone | first6 = M. | year = 2014 | title = Positive and extensive intergroup contact in the past buffers against the disproportionate impact of negative contact in the present | url = | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 44 | issue = 6| pages = 548–562 | doi = 10.1002/ejsp.2029 }} External links
1 : Group processes |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。