词条 | Fixation (visual) |
释义 |
MicrosaccadesMicrosaccades, also known as "flicks", are saccades, involuntarily produced during fixation periods. They are the largest and fastest of the fixational eye movements. Like saccades, microsaccades are usually binocular, and conjugate movements with comparable amplitudes and directions in both eyes. In the 1960s, scientists suggested the maximum amplitude for microsaccades should be 12 arcminutes to distinguish microsaccades and saccades.[5] However, further studies have shown that microsaccades can certainly exceed this value.[6] Therefore, amplitude can no longer be used to distinguish microsaccades and saccades. The only way to distinguish microsaccades from saccades is by the time in which they happen: during fixation. Regular saccades are produced during the active exploration of the eye, during non-fixation tasks such as free viewing or visual search. However, microsaccades are distinguished from regular saccades because they are only produced during fixation tasks.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} The circularity of this definition has been the subject of ample criticism.[7] MechanismMoving in a straight-line-fashion, microsaccades have the ability to carry the retinal image from several dozen to several hundred photoreceptor widths. Because they shift the retinal image, microsaccades overcome adaption[5] and generate neural responses to stationary stimuli in visual neurons.[8] These movements might serve the function of maintaining visibility during fixation,[5] or might be related to attentional shifts to objects in the visual field[9] or in memory[10], might help limit binocular fixation disparity,[11] or may serve some combination of those functions. Medical applicationSome neuroscientists believe that microsaccades are potentially important in neurological and ophthalmic diseases since they are strongly related to many features of visual perception, attention, and cognition.[14] Research aimed at finding the purpose of microsaccades began in the 1990s.[12] The development of non-invasive eye-movement-recording devices, the ability to record single-neuron activity in monkeys, and the use of computational processing power in the analysis of dynamic behavior led to advancements in microsaccade research.[8]{{primary source inline|date=November 2018}} Today, there is growing interest in research on microsaccades. Research on microsaccades includes investigating the perceptual effects of microsaccades, recording the neural responses they induce, and tracking the mechanisms behind their oculomotor generation. It has been shown that when fixation is not explicitly enforced as it often occurs in vision research experiments, microsaccades precisely shift gaze to nearby locations of interest.[13] This behavior compensates for non-uniform vision within the foveola.[14] Some studies suggest the use of microsaccades as a diagnostic method for ADHD.[15][16] Adults diagnosed with ADHD but with no medication treatments tend to blink more and make more microsaccades.[16]{{primary source inline|date=October 2018}} Microsaccades are also being explored as diagnostic measures for Progressive supranuclear palsy, Alzheimer's disease, Autism Spectrum Disorder, acute hypoxia, and other conditions.[17] Ocular driftsOcular drift is the fixational eye movement characterized by a smoother, slower, roaming motion of the eye when fixed on an object. The exact movement of ocular drift is often compared to Brownian motion, which is the random motion of a particle suspended in fluid as a result of its collision with the atoms and molecules that comprise that fluid. The movement can also be compared to a random walk, characterized by random and often erratic changes in direction.[23] Ocular drifts occur incessantly during intersaccadic fixation. Although the frequency of ocular drifts is usually lower than the frequency of ocular microtremors (from 0 to 40 Hz compared to from 40 to 100 Hz), it is problematic to distinguish ocular drifts and ocular microtremors. In fact, microtremors might reflect the Brownian engine underlying the drift motion.[18] Resolution of intersaccadic eye movements is technically challenging.[4] MechanismThe motion of ocular drift is related to the processing and encoding of space and time.[19] It is also related to acquiring minute visual details of objects that are stationary, in order for these details to be further processed.[20][21] Recent results have shown that ocular drift reformats the input signal to the retina equalizing (whitening) spatial power at non-zero temporal frequencies across a broad spatial frequency range.[22] Medical applicationOcular drifts were first found to be caused by an instability of the ocular motor system.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} However, more recent findings have shown that there are actually a number of suggested hypotheses as to why ocular drifts occur. First, ocular drifts can be caused by the uncontrollable random movements driven by neuronal or muscular noise.[23] Second, ocular drifts can occur to counter controlled motor variables, namely a motor control loop.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} Lastly, ocular drifts can be driven by retinal information, in a retino-motor control loop.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} When the head is not immobilized, as in daily life and as is often true in eye movement recordings in the laboratory, ocular drifts compensate for the natural fixational instability of the head.[24] Ocular drifts are altered by some neurologic conditions[17] including Tourette syndrome[25] and autism spectrum disorder[26] Ocular microtremorsOcular microtremors (OMTs) are small, quick, and synchronized oscillations of the eyes occurring at frequencies in a range of 40 to 100 Hz, although they typically occur at around 90 Hz in the average healthy individual.{{citation needed|date=March 2019}} They are characterized by their high frequency and minuscule amplitude of just a few arcseconds. Although the function of ocular microtremors is debatable and not fully known, they seem to play a role in processing of high spatial frequencies, which allows for perception of fine detail.[22][27][28] Studies show that ocular microtremors have some promise as a tool for determining the level of consciousness in an individual[29], as well as the progression of some degenerative diseases including Parkinson's disease[30] and multiple sclerosis.[31] MechanismAlthough originally thought to stem from spontaneous firing of motor units, the origin of ocular microtremors is now believed to be in the oculomotor nuclei in the reticular formation of the brainstem.[32] This new insight opened the possibility of using ocular tremors as a gauge for neuronal activity in that region of the central nervous system. More research must be done, but recent studies strongly suggest that decreased activity in the brainstem correlates with decreased frequency of OMTs.[33] Medical applicationSeveral methods of recording have been developed to observe these minuscule events, the most successful being the piezoelectric strain gauge method, which translates eye movement through a latex probe in contact with the eye leading to piezoelectric strain gauge. This method is used in research settings; more practical adaptations of this technology have been developed for use in clinical settings to monitor the depth of anesthesia.[34] Despite the availability of these methods, tremor remains more difficult to measure than other fixational eye movements, and studies addressing medical applications of tremor movements are rare as a result.[17] Some studies have, nevertheless, pointed to the possibility that tremor movements may be useful in assessing the progression of degenerative diseases including Parkinson's disease[30] and multiple sclerosis.[31] See also
References1. ^Pritchard R.M., Heron W., & Hebb D.O. (1960). Visual Perception Approached by the Method of Stabilized Images. Canadian J. Psych., 14, 67–77. 2. ^{{cite journal |last1=Coppola |first1=D |last2=Purves |first2=D |title=The Extraordinarily Rapid Disappearance of Entoptic Images |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA |date=1996 |volume=93 |pages=8001–8004 |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/93/15/8001.full.pdf+html}} 3. ^M. Rucci and M. Poletti. Control and function of fixational eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1:499–518, 2015. 4. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Rucci|first=Michele|author-link=Michele Rucci|last2=McGraw|first2=Paul V.|last3=Krauzlis|first3=Richard J.|date=2016-01-01|title=Fixational eye movements and perception|journal=Vision Research|volume=118|pages=1–4|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2015.12.001|issn=1878-5646|pmid=26686666}} 5. ^1 2 {{Cite journal|last=Collewijn|first=Han|last2=Kowler|first2=Eileen|date=2008-01-01|title=The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=20.1–21|doi=10.1167/8.14.20|issn=1534-7362|pmc=3522523|pmid=19146321}} 6. ^{{Cite journal|last=Troncoso|first=Xoana G.|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Martinez-Conde|first3=Susana|date=2008-01-01|title=Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in|journal=Journal of Vision|volume=8|issue=14|pages=15.1–9|doi=10.1167/8.14.15|issn=1534-7362|pmid=19146316}} 7. ^M. Poletti and M. Rucci. A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions. Vision Research, 118, 83--97, 2016. 8. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Martinez-Conde|first=S.|author-link=Susana Martinez-Conde|last2=Macknik|first2=S. L.|last3=Hubel|first3=D. H.|date=2000-03-01|title=Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys|journal=Nature Neuroscience|volume=3|issue=3|pages=251–258|doi=10.1038/72961|issn=1097-6256|pmid=10700257}} 9. ^{{cite journal |last1=Laubrock |last2=Engbert |last3=Kliegl |title=Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention |journal=Vision Research |volume=45 |issue=6 |pages=721–730 |doi=10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.029|pmid=15639499 |year=2005 }} 10. ^{{cite journal |last1=Martinez-Conde |first1=S |last2=Alexander |first2=R |title=A gaze bias in the mind's eye |journal=Nature Human Behaviour |pages=1 |date=2019 |url=https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-019-0546-1#article-info|doi=10.1038/s41562-019-0546-1 }} 11. ^{{cite journal |last1=Valsecchi |first1=Matteo |last2=Gegenfurtner |first2=Karl R. |title=Control of binocular gaze in a high-precision manual task |journal=Vision Research |volume=110 |issue=Pt B |pages=203–214 |doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.09.005|pmid=25250983 |year=2015 }} 12. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Martinez-Conde|first=Susana|author-link=Susana Martinez-Conde|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Troncoso|first3=Xoana G.|last4=Hubel|first4=David H.|date=2009-09-01|title=Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis|journal=Trends in Neurosciences|volume=32|issue=9|pages=463–475|doi=10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006|issn=1878-108X|pmid=19716186|citeseerx=10.1.1.493.7537}} 13. ^H.-K. Ko, M. Poletti, and M. Rucci. Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task. Nat. Neurosci., 13(12):1549–1553, 2010. 14. ^M. Poletti, C. Listorti, and M. Rucci. Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea. Curr. Biol., 23(17):1691–1695, 2013. 15. ^{{cite journal |last1=Panagiotidi |first1=M |last2=Overton |first2=P |last3=Stafford |first3=T |title=Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits. |journal=J Eye Mov Res |date=2017 |volume=10 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.16910/10.1.6}} 16. ^1 {{cite journal |last1=Fried |last2=Tsitsiashvili |last3=Bonneh |last4=Sterkin |last5=Wygnanski-Jaffe |last6=Epstein |title=ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication. |journal=Vision Res |volume=101 |pages=62–72 |date=2014 |doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004|pmid=24863585 }} 17. ^1 2 {{cite journal |last1=Alexander |first1=Robert |last2=Macknik |first2=Stephen |last3=Martinez-Conde |first3=Susana |title=Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease |journal=Frontiers in Neurology |date=2018 |volume=9 |issue=144 |pages=144 |doi=10.3389/fneur.2018.00144|pmid=29593642 |pmc=5859063 }} 18. ^{{Cite journal|last=Ahissar|first=Ehud|last2=Arieli|first2=Amos|last3=Fried|first3=Moshe|last4=Bonneh|first4=Yoram|date=2016-01-01|title=On the possible roles of microsaccades and drifts in visual perception|journal=Vision Research|volume=118|pages=25–30|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.004|issn=1878-5646|pmid=25535005}} 19. ^ Ahissar, E. and A. Arieli (2001) Figuring space by time. Neuron 32:185-201 DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00466-4 20. ^{{Cite journal|last=Kuang|first=Xutao|last2=Poletti|first2=Martina|last3=Victor|first3=Jonathan D.|last4=Rucci|first4=Michele|author-link4=Michele Rucci|date=2012-03-20|title=Temporal encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation|journal=Current Biology|volume=22|issue=6|pages=510–514|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.050|issn=1879-0445|pmc=3332095|pmid=22342751}} 21. ^ Ahissar, E. and A. Arieli (2012) Seeing via miniature eye movements: A dynamic hypothesis for vision. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 6:89. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2012.00089 22. ^1 M. Rucci and J. D. Victor. The unsteady eye: an information processing stage, not a bug. Trends in Neurosciences, 38:195–206, 2015. 23. ^{{cite book |last1=Carpenter |first1=R. H. S. |title=Movements of the Eyes |date=1988 |publisher=Pion. |location=London |edition=2nd Edn}} 24. ^1 M. Poletti, M. Aytekin, M. Rucci, "Head-eye coordination at a microscopic scale." Current Biology, 25, 3253-3259, 2015. 25. ^{{cite journal |last1=Shaikh |doi=10.1007/s10072-017-3069-4|pmid=28815321|pmc=6246774|title=Fixational eye movements in Tourette syndrome|journal=Neurological Sciences|volume=38|issue=11|pages=1977–1984|year=2017}} 26. ^{{cite journal |doi=10.1111/ejn.12243|pmid=23692590|pmc=4587666|title=Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder|journal=European Journal of Neuroscience|volume=38|issue=1|pages=2125–2138|year=2013|last1=Frey|first1=Hans-Peter|last2=Molholm|first2=Sophie|last3=Lalor|first3=Edmund C|last4=Russo|first4=Natalie N|last5=Foxe|first5=John J}} 27. ^M. Rucci, R. Iovin, M. Poletti, and F. Santini (2007), Miniature Eye Movements Enhance Fine Spatial Detail, Nature. 447(7146), 851-854. 28. ^{{Cite journal|last=Otero-Millan|first=Jorge|last2=Macknik|first2=Stephen L.|last3=Martinez-Conde|first3=Susana|date=2014-01-01|title=Fixational eye movements and binocular vision|journal=Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience|volume=8|pages=52|doi=10.3389/fnint.2014.00052|issn=1662-5145|pmc=4083562|pmid=25071480}} 29. ^{{cite journal |last1=Bolger |first1=C |last2=Sheahan |first2=N |last3=Coakley |first3=D |last4=Malone |first4=J |title=High frequency eye tremor: reliability of measurement |journal=Clinical Physics and Physiological Measurement |date=1992 |volume=13 |issue=2}} 30. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Bolger|first=C.|last2=Bojanic|first2=S.|last3=Sheahan|first3=N. F.|last4=Coakley|first4=D.|last5=Malone|first5=J. F.|date=1999-04-01|title=Ocular microtremor in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease|journal=Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry|volume=66|issue=4|pages=528–531|issn=0022-3050|pmc=1736284|pmid=10201430|doi=10.1136/jnnp.66.4.528}} 31. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Bolger|first=C.|last2=Bojanic|first2=S.|last3=Sheahan|first3=N.|last4=Malone|first4=J.|last5=Hutchinson|first5=M.|last6=Coakley|first6=D.|date=2000-05-01|title=Ocular microtremor (OMT): a new neurophysiological approach to multiple sclerosis|journal=Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry|volume=68|issue=5|pages=639–642|issn=0022-3050|pmc=1736931|pmid=10766897|doi=10.1136/jnnp.68.5.639}} 32. ^{{Cite journal|last=Spauschus|first=A.|last2=Marsden|first2=J.|last3=Halliday|first3=D. M.|last4=Rosenberg|first4=J. R.|last5=Brown|first5=P.|date=1999-06-01|title=The origin of ocular microtremor in man|journal=Experimental Brain Research|volume=126|issue=4|pages=556–562|issn=0014-4819|pmid=10422719|doi=10.1007/s002210050764}} 33. ^{{Cite journal|last=Bojanic|first=S.|last2=Simpson|first2=T.|last3=Bolger|first3=C.|date=2001-04-01|title=Ocular microtremor: a tool for measuring depth of anaesthesia?|journal=British Journal of Anaesthesia|volume=86|issue=4|pages=519–522|issn=0007-0912|pmid=11573625|doi=10.1093/bja/86.4.519}} 34. ^{{Cite journal|last=Bengi|first=H.|last2=Thomas|first2=J. G.|date=1968-03-01|title=Three electronic methods for recording ocular tremor|journal=Medical & Biological Engineering|volume=6|issue=2|pages=171–179|issn=0025-696X|pmid=5651798|doi=10.1007/bf02474271}} 2 : Eye|Vision |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。