请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Chimpanzee
释义

  1. Etymology

  2. Taxonomy and genetics

     Evolution  Subspecies  Genome 

  3. Description

  4. Ecology

     Diet  Mortality and health 

  5. Behaviour

     Group structure  Mating and parenting  Communication  Hunting 

  6. Intelligence and cognition

     Tool use  Memory  Cooperation  Laughter  Language 

  7. Relations with humans

     In culture  As pets  Use in research  Field study  Attacks  Link with human immunodeficiency virus type 1  Status and conservation 

  8. See also

  9. References

  10. External links

{{other uses}}{{Use dmy dates|date=January 2019}}{{Speciesbox
| name = Common chimpanzee[1]
| fossil_range = {{fossilrange|4|0|earliest=6}}
| status = EN
| status_system = IUCN3.1
| status_ref = [2]
| image = Chimpanzee (3265647592).jpg
| genus = Pan
| species = troglodytes
| authority = (Oken, 1816)
| range_map = Pan troglodytes area.png
| range_map_caption = distribution of subspecies
1. Pan troglodytes verus (green)
2. P. t. ellioti (gray)
3. P. t. troglodytes (red)
4. P. t. schweinfurthii (blue)
| subdivision =Pan troglodytes troglodytes
Pan troglodytes verus
Pan troglodytes ellioti
Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii
| synonyms =Simia troglodytes Blumenbach, 1776
Troglodytes troglodytes (Blumenbach, 1776)
Troglodytes niger E. Geoffroy, 1812
Pan niger (E. Geoffroy, 1812)Anthropopithecus troglodytes (Sutton, 1883)
}}

The chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), also known as the common chimpanzee, robust chimpanzee, or simply "chimp," is a species of great ape, with four confirmed subspecies and a fifth proposed subspecies. The chimpanzee, along with the closely related bonobo (sometimes called the pygmy chimpanzee), is a species in the genus Pan. Evidence from fossils and DNA sequencing shows that the genus Pan is a sister taxon to the human lineage and are humans' closet living relatives.

The chimpanzee is covered in coarse black hair, but has a bare face, fingers, toes, palms of the hands, and soles of the feet. It is considered more robust than the bonobo, weighing between {{convert |40 |and |65 |kg |abbr=on}} and measuring about {{convert |63 |to |94 |cm |in |round=0.5 |abbr=on}}. Its gestation period is eight months. The infant is weaned at about three years old, but usually maintains a close relationship with its mother for several more years. It lives in groups which range in size from 15 to 150 members, although individuals travel and forage in much smaller groups during the day. The species lives in a male-dominated, strict hierarchy, which means disputes can generally be settled without the need for violence. Nearly all chimpanzee populations have been recorded using tools, modifying sticks, rocks, grass and leaves and using them for acquiring honey, termites, ants, nuts and water. The species has also been found creating sharpened sticks to spear small mammals.

The chimpanzee is listed on the IUCN Red List as an endangered species. Between 170,000 and 300,000 individuals are estimated across its range in the forests and savannahs of West and Central Africa. The biggest threats to the chimpanzee are habitat loss, poaching and disease. Chimpanzees appear in Western popular culture as stereotyped clown-figures, and have featured in entertainments such as chimpanzees' tea parties, circus acts and stage shows. They are sometimes kept as pets, though their strength and aggressiveness makes them dangerous in this role. Some hundreds have been kept in laboratories for research, especially in America. Many attempts have been made to teach languages such as American Sign Language to chimpanzees, with doubtful success.

Etymology

The English name "chimpanzee" is first recorded in 1738. It is derived from Vili ci-mpenze[3] or Tshiluba language chimpenze,[4] with a meaning of "mockman" or possibly just "ape". The colloquialism "chimp" was most likely coined some time in the late 1870s.[5]

Taxonomy and genetics

The first great ape known to Western science in the 17th century was the "Orang-Outang", the local Malay name being recorded in Java by the Dutch physician Jacobus Bontius.[6] In 1641, the Dutch anatomist Nicolaes Tulp applied the name to a chimpanzee or bonobo brought to the Netherlands from Angola.[6] Another Dutch anatomist, Peter Camper, dissected specimens from Central Africa and Southeast Asia in the 1770s, noting the differences between the African and Asian apes. The German naturalist Johann Friedrich Blumenbach classified the common chimpanzee as Simia troglodytes by in 1776. The name troglodytes was taken from Troglodytae, a mythical race of cave-dwellers. Another German naturalist, Lorenz Oken, coined the genus Pan, from the Greek god, in 1816.[7][8]

Evolution

{{see |Chimpanzee–human last common ancestor}}

Despite a large number of Homo fossil finds, chimpanzee fossils (genus Pan) were not described until 2005. Existing chimpanzee populations in West and Central Africa do not overlap with the major human fossil sites in East Africa. However, chimpanzee fossils have now been reported from Kenya. This would indicate that both humans and members of the Pan clade were present in the East African Rift Valley during the Middle Pleistocene.[10]

DNA evidence suggests the bonobo and common chimpanzee species separated from each other less than one million years ago (similar in relation between Homo sapiens and Neanderthals).[11][12] A 2017 genetic study suggests ancient gene flow (introgression) between 200 and 550 thousand years ago from the bonobo into the ancestors of central and eastern chimpanzees.[13] The chimpanzee line split from the last common ancestor of the human line around six million years ago. Because no species other than Homo sapiens has survived from the human line of that branching, both chimpanzee species are the closest living relatives of humans; the lineage of humans and chimpanzees diverged from genus Gorilla about seven million years ago. A 2003 study argues the common chimpanzee should be included in the human branch as Homo troglodytes, and notes "experts say many scientists are likely to resist the reclassification, especially in the emotionally-charged and often disputed field of anthropology".[14]

Subspecies

Four subspecies of the common chimpanzee have been recognised,[15][16] with the possibility of a fifth:[13][17]

  • Central chimpanzee or tschego, Pan troglodytes troglodytes, in Cameroon, the Central African Republic, Equatorial Guinea, Gabon, the Republic of the Congo, and the Democratic Republic of the Congo
  • Western chimpanzee, P. troglodytes verus, in Guinea, Guinea-Bissau, Mali, Senegal, Sierra Leone, Liberia, Ivory Coast, and Ghana
  • Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee, P. troglodytes ellioti (also known as P. t. vellerosus),[15] in Nigeria and Cameroon
  • Eastern chimpanzee, P. troglodytes schweinfurthii, in the Central African Republic, South Sudan, the Democratic Republic of the Congo, Uganda, Rwanda, Burundi, Tanzania, and Zambia
  • Southeastern chimpanzee, P. troglodytes marungensis, in Burundi, Rwanda, Tanzania, and Uganda: Colin Groves argues that this is a subspecies, created by enough variation between the northern and southern populations of P. t. schweinfurthii.[17]

Genome

{{Main |Chimpanzee genome project}}{{Infobox genome | taxId=202 | ploidy=diploid | chromosomes=24 pairs | size=3,323.27 Mb | year =}}Human and common chimpanzee DNA is very similar. A Chimpanzee Genome Project was initiated after the completion of the Human Genome Project. In December 2003, a preliminary analysis of 7600 genes shared between the two genomes confirmed that certain genes, such as the forkhead-box P2 transcription factor which is involved in speech development, have undergone rapid evolution in the human lineage. A draft version of the chimpanzee genome was published on 1 September 2005 by the Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium.[18]

The DNA sequence differences between humans and chimpanzees consist of about 35 million single-nucleotide changes, five million insertion/deletion events, and various chromosomal rearrangements. Typical human and chimp protein homologs differ in an average of only two amino acids. About 30% of all human proteins are identical in sequence to the corresponding chimp protein. Duplications of small parts of chromosomes have been the major source of differences between human and chimp genetic material; about 2.7% of the corresponding modern genomes represent differences, produced by gene duplications or deletions, since humans and chimps diverged from their common evolutionary ancestor. Results from human and chimp genome analyses, currently being conducted by geneticists including David Reich, should help in understanding the genetic basis of some human diseases. Research into genetic conditions in wild chimpanzees has also been undertaken.[19]

Description

Adult males weigh between {{convert|40|and|60|kg|abbr=on}}; females weigh {{convert|32|to|47|kg|abbr=on}}.[20] However, large wild males can weigh up to {{convert|70|kg|lb|abbr=on}}, and males in captivity, such as Travis the Chimp, have reached {{convert|91|kg|lb|abbr=on}}.[21][22][23] Head-body length (from the nose to the rump while on all fours) ranges from {{convert|63|to|94|cm|in|abbr=on}}.[23][24] Males can measure up to {{convert|1.6|m|abbr=on}} tall while standing and females up to {{convert|1.3 |m |abbr=on}} tall. Their bodies are covered by coarse, black hair, except for the face, fingers, toes, palms of the hands, and soles of the feet. Both its thumbs and big toes are opposable, allowing a precise grip. The common chimpanzee is both arboreal and terrestrial, and spends its nights in the trees, while most daylight hours are spent on the ground.[25]

The habitual gait is quadrupedal, using the soles of the feet and resting on the knuckles, but they can walk upright for short distances. The common chimpanzee is a 'knuckle walker', like the gorilla and the bonobo,[25] in contrast to the quadrupedal locomotion of the orangutan, a 'palm walker' that uses the outside edge of its palms. It is anatomically the closest relative of the human.{{cn|date=March 2019}}

Ecology

The common chimpanzee is a highly adaptable species. It lives in a variety of habitats, including dry savanna, evergreen rainforest, montane forest, swamp forest and dry woodland-savanna mosaic.[26][33] In Gombe, the chimpanzee lives in subalpine moorland, open woodland, semideciduous forest, evergreen forest, and grassland with scattered trees.[33] At Bossou, the chimpanzee inhabits multistage secondary deciduous forests, which have grown after shifting cultivation, as well as primary forests and grasslands.[27] At Taï, it can be found in the last remaining tropical rain forest in Ivory Coast.[28] The chimpanzee has an advanced cognitive map of its home range and can repeatedly find food.[33] The chimpanzee makes a night nest in a tree in a new location every night, with every chimpanzee in a separate nest other than infants or juvenile chimpanzees, which sleep with their mothers.[38]

Diet

The chimpanzee is an omnivorous frugivore. It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant. It also eats leaves and leaf buds, seeds, blossoms, stems, pith, bark and resin. Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet, estimated as 2%.[33][29] While the common chimpanzee is mostly herbivorous, it does eat honey, soil, insects, birds and their eggs, and small to medium-sized mammals, including other primates.[33][30] The western red colobus ranks at the top of preferred mammal prey. Other mammalian prey include red-tailed monkeys, yellow baboons, bush babies, blue duikers, bushbucks, and common warthogs.[31] Isolated cases of cannibalism have also been documented.[32]

Despite the fact that common chimpanzees are known to hunt, and to collect insects and other invertebrates, such food actually makes up a tiny portion of their diet, from as little as 2% yearly to as much as 65 grams of animal flesh per day for each adult chimpanzee in peak hunting seasons. This also varies from troop to troop and year to year. However, in all cases, the majority of their diet consists of fruits, leaves, roots, and other plant matter.[33][34] Female chimpanzees appear to consume much less animal flesh than males, according to several studies.[35] Jane Goodall documented many occasions within Gombe Stream National Park of chimpanzees and western red colobus monkeys ignoring each other within close proximity.[36][37]

Mortality and health

The average lifespan of a chimpanzee is usually less than 15 years, although individual that reach 12 years may live an additional 15. Individual may exceed 27 years and even over 60. Captive chimps live longer with median lifespans of 31.7 years for males and 38.7 years for females. Captive chimps have been recorded to live up to 72 years.[38]

Leopard predation is a significant cause of mortality in chimpanzees at Taï and Lopé National Parks.[28][39] Chimps are generally hostile towards leopards and may mob the predators and even kill their cubs.[40] Lions may have preyed on the chimpanzees at Mahale Mountains National Park, where at least four chimpanzees could have fallen prey to lions. Although no other instances of lion predation on chimpanzees have been recorded, the larger group sizes of savanna chimps may have developed as a response to threats from these big cats.[41]

Chimps and humans share only 50% of their parasite and microbe species. This is due to the differences in environmental and dietary adaptations with human internal parasite species overlapping more with omnivorous, savanna dwelling baboons. The chimpanzee is host to the louse species Pediculus schaeffi which is a close relative of Pediculus humanus which infests human head and body hair. By contrast, the human pubic louse Pthirus pubis is closely related to Pthirus gorillae which infests gorillas.[42] A 2017 study of gastrointestinal parasites of wild chimps in degraded forests in Uganda found nine species of protozoa, five nematodes, one cestode, and one trematode. The most prevalent species being the protozoa Troglodytella abrassarti.[43]

Behaviour

It is suspected that human observers influence chimpanzee behaviour. It is suggested that drones, camera traps and remote microphones should be used rather than human observers.[44]{{better source needed|date=March 2019}}

Group structure

Common chimpanzees live in communities that typically range from 20 to more than 150 members, but spend most of their time traveling in small, temporary groups consisting of a few individuals, which may consist of any combination of age and sex classes. Both males and females sometimes travel alone.[45] The common chimpanzee lives in a fission-fusion society and may be found in groups of these types: all-male, adult females and offspring, both sexes, or one female and her offspring. Chimpanzees have complex social relationships and spend a large amount of time grooming each other.[46]

At the core of social structures are males, which roam around, protect group members, and search for food. Males remain in their natal communities, while females generally emigrate at adolescence. As such, males in a community are more likely to be related to one another than females are to each other. Among males is generally a dominance hierarchy, and males are dominant over females.[47] However, this unusual fission-fusion social structure, "in which portions of the parent group may on a regular basis separate from and then rejoin the rest,"[48] is highly variable in terms of which particular individual chimpanzees congregate at a given time. This is caused mainly by the large measure of individual autonomy that individuals have within their fission-fusion social groups, though the large ranges of communities that overlap with those of other groups may also contribute.

As a result, individual chimpanzees often forage for food alone, or in smaller groups (as opposed to the much larger "parent" group, which encompasses all the chimpanzees which regularly come into contact and congregate into parties in a particular area). As stated, these smaller groups also emerge in a variety of types, for a variety of purposes. For example, an all-male troop may be organized to hunt for meat, while a group consisting of lactating females serves to act as a "nursery group" for the young.[49] An individual may encounter certain individuals quite frequently, but have run-ins with others almost never or only in large-scale gatherings. Due to the varying frequency at which chimpanzees associate, the structure of their societies is highly complicated.

Male chimpanzees exist in a linear dominance hierarchy. Top-ranking males tend to be aggressive even during dominance stability.[50] This is likely due to the chimp’s fission-fusion society, with male chimps leaving groups and returning after extended periods of time. With this, a dominant male is unsure if any "political maneuvering" has occurred and must re-establish his dominance. Thus, a large amount of aggression occurs 5–15 minutes after a reunion. During aggressive encounters, displays are preferred over attacks.[50][51]

Males maintain and improve their social ranks by forming coalitions, which have been characterized as "exploitative" and are based on an individual's influence in agonistic interactions.[52] Being in a coalition allows males to dominate a third individual when they could not by themselves, as politically apt chimps can exert power over aggressive interactions regardless of their rank. Coalitions can also give an individual male the confidence to challenge a dominant male. The more allies a male has, the better his chance of becoming dominant. However, most changes in hierarchical rank are caused by dyadic interactions.[50] Chimpanzee alliances can be very fickle and one member may turn on another if it serves him.[53]

Low-ranking males commonly switch sides in disputes between more dominant individuals. Low-ranking males benefit from an unstable hierarchy and have increased sexual opportunities.[52][53] In addition, conflicts between dominant males cause them to focus on each other rather than the lower-ranking males. Social hierarchies among adult females tend to be weaker. Nevertheless, the status of an adult female may be important for her offspring.[71] Females in Taï have also been recorded to form alliances.[54] Social grooming appears to be important in the formation and maintenance of coalitions.[55] It is more common among adult males than adult females.

Chimpanzees have been described as highly territorial and are known to kill other chimps,[56] although Margaret Power wrote in her 1991 book The Egalitarians that the field studies from which the aggressive data came, Gombe and Mahale, use artificial feeding systems that increased aggression in the chimpanzee populations studied, so might not reflect innate characteristics of the species as a whole.[75] In the years following her artificial feeding conditions at Gombe, Jane Goodall described groups of male chimps patrolling the borders of their territory, brutally attacking chimps which had split off from the Gombe group. A study published in 2010 found that the chimpanzees wage wars over land, not mates.[57] Patrol parties from smaller groups are more likely to avoid contact with their neighbors. Patrol parties from large groups even take over a smaller group's territory, gaining access to more resources, food, and females.[48][53]

Mating and parenting

Chimpanzees mate throughout the year, although the number of females in oestrus varies seasonally in a group.[28][58] Female chimps are more likely to come into oestrus when food is readily available. Oestrous females exhibit sexual swellings. Chimps tend to be promiscuous, and during estrus females mate with several males in her community,[48] and males have large testicles for sperm competition. However, other forms of mating also exist. A community's dominant males sometimes restrict reproductive access to females. A male and female can form consortship and mate outside their community. In addition, females sometimes leave their communities and mate with males from neighboring communities.[48][59]

These alternative mating strategies give females more mating opportunities without losing the support of the males in their community.[59] Infanticide has been recorded in chimp communities in Gombe, Mahale, and Kibale National Parks. Male chimps practice infanticide on unrelated young to shorten the interbirth intervals in the females. Also, accounts of infanticide by females have been reported; cases of female infanticide may be related to the dominance hierarchy in females or simply isolated pathological behaviors.[48][60]

Care for the young is provided mostly by their mothers. The survival and emotional health of the young is dependent on maternal care.[48] Mothers provide their young with food, warmth, and protection, and teach them certain skills. In addition, a chimp’s future rank may be dependent on its mother’s status.[48][28] For their first 30 days, infants cling to their mother's bellies. Newborn chimps are helpless; their grasping reflex is not strong enough to support them for more than a few seconds. Infants are unable to support their own weight for their first two months and need their mothers' support.[61]

When they reach five to six months, infants ride on their mothers' backs. They remain in continual contact for the rest of their first year. When they reach two years of age, they are able to move and sit independently. By three years, infants move farther away from their mothers. By four to six years, chimps are weaned and infancy ends.[61]

The juvenile period for chimps lasts from their sixth to ninth years. Juveniles remain close to their mothers, but they also have more interactions with other members of their community. Adolescent females move between groups and are supported by their mothers in agonistic encounters. Adolescent males spend time with adult males in social activities like hunting and boundary patrolling.[61]

Communication

{{listen
| filename = Pant-hoot call made by a male chimpanzee.ogg
| title = Chimpanzee pant-hoot call
| description = Pant-hoot call made by an adult male chimpanzee, effectively demonstrating the introduction, build-up, climax and let-down phases.[62]
| format = Vorbis
}}

Chimpanzees use facial expressions, postures and sounds to communicate with each other. Chimps have expressive faces which are important in close-up communications. When frightened, a "full closed grin" causes nearby individuals to be fearful, as well. Other facial expressions include the "lip flip", "pout", "sneer", and "compressed-lips face". When submitting to a conspecific, a chimp crunches, bobs, and extends a hand. When in an aggressive mode, a chimp swaggers bipedally, hunched over and arms waving, in an attempt to exaggerate its size.[48] Chimps beat their hands and feet against the trunks of large trees, an act known as "drumming".[63]

Vocalizations are also important in chimp communication. The most common and important call in adults is the "pant-hoot". These calls are made when individuals are excited.[48] Pant-hoots are made of four parts, starting with soft "hoos" that get louder and louder and climax into screams and sometimes barks; the former die down to soft "hoos" again as the call ends.[63] Submissive individuals will make "pant-grunts" towards their superiors.[48][60] Chimps use distance calls to draw attention to danger, food sources, or other community members.[48] "Barks" may be made as "short barks" when hunting and "tonal barks" when sighting large snakes.[63]

Hunting

When hunting small monkeys such as the red colobus, the chimpanzee hunts where the forest canopy is interrupted or irregular. This allows it to easily corner the monkeys when chasing them in the appropriate direction. Chimps may also hunt as a coordinated team, so that they can corner their prey even in a continuous canopy. During an arboreal hunt, each chimp in the hunting groups has a role. "Drivers" serve to keep the prey running in a certain direction and follow them without attempting to make a catch. "Blockers" are stationed at the bottom of the trees and climb up to block prey that take off in a different direction. "Chasers" move quickly and try to make a catch. Finally, "ambushers" hide and rush out when a monkey nears.[64]

While both adults and infants are taken, adult male black-and-white colobus monkeys will attack the hunting chimps. In Gombe, the chimpanzee also fears adult female colobus monkeys, and prefers to snatch infants from their mother's bellies without harming the mothers.[48] Male chimps hunt more than females. When caught and killed, the meal is distributed to all hunting party members and even bystanders.[64]

Intelligence and cognition

Tool use

{{further |Tool use by animals}}

Nearly all chimpanzee populations have been recorded using tools. They modify sticks, rocks, grass, and leaves and use them when foraging for honey, termites, ants, nuts, and water. Despite the lack of complexity, forethought and skill are seen in making these tools and should be considered such.[65] While it has been known since Jane Goodall's 1960s discovery that modern chimpanzees use tools, research published in 2007 indicates chimpanzee stone tool use dates to at least 4,300 years ago.[66]

A common chimpanzee from the Kasakela chimpanzee community was the first nonhuman animal reported making a tool, by modifying a twig to use as an instrument for extracting termites from their mound.[67][68][69] At Taï, chimps simply use their hands to extract termites.[65] When foraging for honey, chimps use modified short sticks to scoop the honey out of the hive, that is, if the bees are stingless. For hives of the dangerous African honeybees, chimps use longer and thinner sticks to extract the honey.[70] Chimps also fish for ants using the same tactic.[48][65]

Ant dipping is difficult and some chimps never master it. West African chimps crack open hard nuts with stones or branches.[28][65] Some forethought in this activity is apparent, as these items are not found together or near a source of nuts. Nut cracking is also difficult and must be learned.[28] Chimps also use leaves as sponges or spoons to drink water.[71]

A recent study revealed the use of such advanced tools as spears, which West African chimpanzees in Senegal sharpen with their teeth, being used to spear Senegal bushbabies out of small holes in trees.[72] An eastern chimpanzee has been observed using a modified branch as a tool to capture a squirrel.[73]

Memory

A 30-year study at Kyoto University's Primate Research Institute has shown that chimps are able to learn to recognise the numbers 1 through 9 and their values. The chimps further show an aptitude for photographic memory, demonstrated in experiments in which the jumbled digits are flashed onto a computer screen for less than a quarter of a second. One chimp, Ayumu, was able to correctly and quickly point to the positions where they appeared in ascending order. The same experiment was failed by human world memory champion Ben Pridmore on most attempts.[74]

Cooperation

In controlled cooperative experiments researchers have found that chimpanzees have a basic understanding of cooperation. Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators.[75] In a group setting with a device that delivered food rewards only to cooperating chimpanzees, cooperation first increased, then, due to competitive behaviour, decreased, before finally increasing to the highest level through punishment and other arbitrage behaviour.[76]

Laughter

{{See also |Laughter in animals}}

Chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans show laughter-like vocalizations in response to physical contact, such as wrestling, play-chasing, or tickling. This is documented in wild and captive chimpanzees. Common chimpanzee laughter is not readily recognisable to humans as such, because it is generated by alternating inhalations and exhalations that sound more like breathing and panting. Instances in which nonhuman primates have expressed joy have been reported. One study analyzed and recorded sounds made by human babies and bonobos when tickled. Although the bonobo's laugh was a higher frequency, the laugh followed a pattern similar to that of human babies and included similar facial expressions. Humans and chimpanzees share similar ticklish areas of the body, such as the armpits and belly. The enjoyment of tickling in chimpanzees does not diminish with age.[77]

Language

{{Main |Great ape language}}

Scientists have attempted to teach human language to several species of great apes. One early attempt by Allen and Beatrix Gardner in the 1960s involved spending 51 months teaching American Sign Language to a chimpanzee named Washoe. The Gardners reported that Washoe learned 151 signs, and had spontaneously taught them to other chimpanzees.[78] Over a longer period of time, Washoe was reported to have learned over 800 signs.[79]

Debate is ongoing among scientists such as David Premack about chimpanzees' ability to learn language. Since the early reports on Washoe, numerous other studies have been conducted, with varying levels of success.[80] One involved a chimpanzee jokingly named Nim Chimpsky (in allusion to the theorist of language Noam Chomsky), trained by Herbert Terrace of Columbia University. Although his initial reports were quite positive, in November 1979, Terrace and his team, including psycholinguist Thomas Bever, re-evaluated the videotapes of Nim with his trainers, analyzing them frame by frame for signs, as well as for exact context (what was happening both before and after Nim's signs). In the reanalysis, Terrace and Bever concluded that Nim's utterances could be explained merely as prompting on the part of the experimenters, as well as mistakes in reporting the data. "Much of the apes' behaviour is pure drill," he said. "Language still stands as an important definition of the human species." In this reversal, Terrace now argued Nim's use of ASL was not like human language acquisition. Nim never initiated conversations himself, rarely introduced new words, and simply imitated what the humans did. More importantly, Nim's word strings varied in their ordering, suggesting that he was incapable of syntax. Nim's sentences also did not grow in length, unlike human children whose vocabulary and sentence length show a strong positive correlation.[81]

Relations with humans

In culture

Chimpanzees are rarely represented in African culture, as people regard them as too close to humans for comfort. The Gio people of Liberia and the Hemba people of the Congo have created masks of the animals with a smile which suggests drunken anger, insanity or horror. They wear these masks when teaching young people how not to behave. They may also act out these rituals during funerals, representing the "awful reality of death". Stories have been told of chimps kidnapping and raping women.[82]

In Western popular culture, chimpanzees have been stereotyped as childlike companions, sidekicks or clowns. They are especially suited for the latter role on account of their prominent facial features, long limbs and fast movements, which humans often find amusing. Accordingly, entertainment acts featuring chimpanzees dressed up as humans have been traditional staples of circuses and stage shows.[83] Westerners have been disturbed by the chimps' resemblance to humans as well as their "frank sexuality".[82]

From 1926 until 1972, London Zoo, followed by several other zoos around the world, held a chimpanzees' tea party daily, inspiring a long-running series of advertisements for PG Tips tea featuring such a party; the chimpanzees were given speech, lip-synchronized for humorous effect by actors such as Peter Sellers and Bob Monkhouse.[84][85] Similar acts later appeared on television: series whose cast consisted entirely of chimpanzees dressed as humans and "speaking" lines dubbed by human actors. These shows, such as Lancelot Link, Secret Chimp in the 1970s or The Chimp Channel in the 1990s, relied on the novelty of performing apes to carry their timeworn, low comedy gags.[83] Animal rights groups have urged a stop to this practice, considering it abusive.[86] J. Fred Muggs appeared with Today Show host Dave Garroway in the 1950s, Judy on Daktari in the 1960s and Darwin on The Wild Thornberrys in the 1990s. In contrast to the fictional depictions of other animals, such as dogs (as in Lassie), dolphins (Flipper), horses (The Black Stallion) or even other great apes (King Kong), chimpanzee characters and actions are rarely relevant to the plot.[83]

Depictions of chimpanzees as individuals rather than stock characters, and as central rather than incidental to the plot can be found in science fiction. Robert A. Heinlein's short story "Jerry Was a Man" (1947) centers on a genetically enhanced chimpanzee suing for better treatment. The 1972 film Conquest of the Planet of the Apes, the third sequel of Planet of the Apes, portrays a futuristic revolt of enslaved apes led by the only talking chimpanzee, Caesar, against their human masters.[83]

As pets

Chimpanzees have traditionally been kept as pets in a few African villages, especially in the Democratic Republic of Congo. In Virunga National Park in the east of the country, the park authorities regularly confiscate chimpanzees from people keeping them as pets.[87] Outside their range, chimpanzees are popular as wild pets despite their strength and aggression. Even where keeping non-human primates as pets is illegal, the exotic pet trade continues to prosper, leading to injuries from attacks.[88]

Use in research

{{see also |Animal testing on non-human primates#Chimpanzees in the U.S.}}

Hundreds of chimpanzees have been kept in laboratories for research. Most such laboratories either conduct or make the animals available for invasive research,[89] defined as "inoculation with an infectious agent, surgery or biopsy conducted for the sake of research and not for the sake of the chimpanzee, and/or drug testing".[90] Two federally funded American laboratories use chimps: the Yerkes National Primate Research Center at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, and the Southwest National Primate Center in San Antonio, Texas.[91] Five hundred chimps have been retired from laboratory use in the U.S. and live in animal sanctuaries in the U.S. or Canada.[89]

Chimpanzees used in biomedical research tend to be used repeatedly over decades, rather than used and killed as with most laboratory animals. Some individual chimps currently in American laboratories have been used in experiments for over 40 years.[92]

With the publication of the chimpanzee genome, plans to increase the use of chimps in labs are reportedly increasing, with some scientists arguing that the federal moratorium on breeding chimps for research should be lifted.[91][93] A five-year moratorium was imposed by the US National Institutes of Health in 1996, because too many chimps had been bred for HIV research, and it has been extended annually since 2001.[91]

Other researchers argue that chimps either should not be used in research, or should be treated differently. Pascal Gagneux, an evolutionary biologist and primate expert at the University of California, San Diego, argues, given chimpanzees' sense of self, tool use, and genetic similarity to human beings, studies using chimps should follow the ethical guidelines used for human subjects unable to give consent.[91] A recent study suggests chimpanzees which are retired from labs exhibit a form of posttraumatic stress disorder.[94] Stuart Zola, director of the Yerkes National Primate Research Laboratory, disagrees. He told National Geographic: "I don't think we should make a distinction between our obligation to treat humanely any species, whether it's a rat or a monkey or a chimpanzee. No matter how much we may wish it, chimps are not human."[91]

Field study

Jane Goodall undertook the first long-term field study of the common chimpanzee, begun in Tanzania at Gombe Stream National Park in 1960.[67][68] Other long-term studies begun in 1960 include A. Kortlandt's in the eastern Democratic Republic of the Congo and Junichiro Itani's in Mahale Mountains National Park in Tanzania.[95] Current understanding of the species' typical behaviours and social organization has been formed largely from Goodall's ongoing 50-year Gombe research study.[96]

Attacks

Common chimpanzees have attacked humans.[97][98] In Uganda, several attacks on children have happened, some of them fatal. Some of these attacks may be due to the chimpanzees being intoxicated (from alcohol obtained from rural brewing operations) and mistaking human children[99] for the western red colobus, one of their favorite meals.[100] Human interactions with chimpanzees may be especially dangerous if the chimpanzees perceive humans as potential rivals.[101] At least six cases of chimpanzees snatching and eating human babies are documented.[102]

A chimpanzee's strength and sharp teeth mean that attacks, even on adult humans, can cause severe injuries. This was evident after the attack and near death of former NASCAR driver St. James Davis, who was mauled by two escaped chimps while he and his wife were celebrating the birthday of their former pet chimp.[103][104] Another example of chimpanzees being aggressive toward humans occurred in 2009 in Stamford, Connecticut, when a {{convert |200 |lb |adj=on}}, 13-year-old pet chimp named Travis attacked his owner's friend, who lost her hands, eyes, nose, and part of her maxilla from the attack.[105][106]

Link with human immunodeficiency virus type 1

Two types of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infect humans: HIV-1 and HIV-2. HIV-1 is the more virulent and easily transmitted, and is the source of the majority of HIV infections throughout the world; HIV-2 is largely confined to west Africa.[107] Both types originated in west and central Africa, jumping from primates to humans. HIV-1 has evolved from a simian immunodeficiency virus (SIVcpz) found in the common chimpanzee subspecies, P. t. troglodytes, native to southern Cameroon.[108][109] Kinshasa, in the Democratic Republic of Congo, has the greatest genetic diversity of HIV-1 so far discovered, suggesting the virus has been there longer than anywhere else. HIV-2 crossed species from a different strain of HIV, found in the sooty mangabey monkeys in Guinea-Bissau.[107]

Status and conservation

Chimpanzee are a legally protected species in most of their range and can be found both in and outside national parks. Between 172,700 and 299,700 individuals are thought to be living in the wild,[2] a decrease from about a million chimpanzees in the early 1900s.[169]

The biggest threats to the common chimpanzee are habitat destruction, poaching, and disease. Chimpanzee habitats have been limited by deforestation in both West and Central Africa. Road building has caused habitat degradation and fragmentation of chimpanzee populations and may allow poachers more access to areas that had not been seriously affected by humans. While deforestation rates are low in western Central Africa, selective logging may be done outside national parks.[2]

Chimpanzees are a common target for poachers. In Ivory Coast, chimpanzees make up 1–3% of bushmeat sold in urban markets. They are also taken for the pet trade, despite its being illegal in many countries where they live. Chimpanzees are also hunted for medicinal purposes in some areas. People sometimes kill chimpanzees that threaten their crops. Chimps may also be unintentionally maimed or killed by snares meant for other animals.[2]

Infectious diseases are a main cause of death for chimpanzees. They succumb to many diseases that afflict humans, because the two species are so similar. As human populations grow, so does the risk of disease transmission between humans and chimpanzees.[2]

On 12 June 2015, the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service announced it will classify all chimpanzees, both wild and captive, as endangered under the Endangered Species Act. Before this ruling, only wild chimpanzees were listed as endangered, while captive chimpanzees were listed as threatened under the act.[173][110] The final rule was published in the Federal Register of 16 June 2015 ({{Federal Register |80 |34499}}), and came into effect 90 days after publication on 14 September 2015.[111]

See also

{{Commons+cat |Pan troglodytes |Pan_troglodytes}}{{Portal |Animals |Mammals |Primates |Africa}}
  • Anthropopithecus
  • Bili ape
  • Prostitution among animals
  • Great ape personhood
  • Theory of mind
  • The Third Chimpanzee
General:
  • Homininae
  • List of apes – list of notable individuals

References

1. ^{{MSW3 Groves|pages=183|id=12100798}}
2. ^{{cite journal |author=Humle, T. |author2=Maisels, F. |author3=Oates, J.F. |author4=Plumptre, A. |author5=Williamson, E.A. | title=Pan troglodytes | journal=IUCN Red List of Threatened Species | volume=2016 | page=e.T15933A17964454 | year=2016 | url=http://oldredlist.iucnredlist.org/details/15933/0 | access-date=5 September 2016}}
3. ^{{cite book |title=American Heritage Dictionary |date=2011 |edition=5th |chapter-url=https://www.ahdictionary.com/word/search.html?q=chimpanzee |access-date=31 August 2018 |chapter=chimpanzee |publisher=Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company}}
4. ^{{cite web |title=chimpanzee |url=https://www.etymonline.com/word/chimpanzee#etymonline_v_11268 |website=Online Etymology Dictionary |access-date=31 August 2018}}
5. ^{{cite web |url=http://dictionary.reference.com/browse/chimp |title=chimp definition | Dictionary.com |publisher=Dictionary.reference.com |access-date=2009-06-06}}
6. ^{{cite journal |last1=van Wyhe |first1=John |last2=Kjærgaard |first2=Peter C. |title=Going the whole orang: Darwin, Wallace and the natural history of orangutans |journal=Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences |volume=51 |year=2015 |pages=53–63 |doi=10.1016/j.shpsc.2015.02.006}}
7. ^{{citebook|author=Corbey, Raymond |year=2005 |title=The Metaphysics of Apes: Negotiating the Animal-Human Boundary |publisher=Cambridge University Press |page=42–51 |isbn=9780521836838}}
8. ^{{cite book |author=Stanford, Craig |year=2018 |title=The New Chimpanzee, A Twenty-First-Century Portrait of Our Closest Kin |publisher=Harvard University Press |pages=176 |isbn=978-0-674-97711-2}}
9. ^{{cite journal |last1=Goldman |first=D. |last2= Giri |first2=P. R. |last3= O'Brien |first3=S. J. | title=A molecular phylogeny of the hominoid primates as indicated by two-dimensional protein electrophoresis | journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume=84 | issue=10 | pages=3307–3311 | date=May 1987 | pmid=3106965 | pmc=304858 | doi=10.1073/pnas.84.10.3307 | bibcode=1987PNAS...84.3307G}}
10. ^{{cite journal |last1=McBrearty |first1=Sally |last2=Jablonski |first2=Nina G. |date=1 September 2005 |title=First fossil chimpanzee |journal=Nature |pmid=16136135 |doi=10.1038/nature04008 |bibcode=2005Natur.437..105M |volume=437 |issue=7055 |pages=105–8}}
11. ^{{cite journal |author1=Won, Y.J. |author2=Hey, J. | title=Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees | journal=Molecular Biology and Evolution | volume=22 | issue=2 | pages=297–307 | date=February 2005 | pmid=15483319 | doi=10.1093/molbev/msi017}}
12. ^{{cite journal |author1=Fischer, A. |author2=Wiebe, V. |author3=Pääbo, S. |author4=Przeworski, M. | title=Evidence for a complex demographic history of chimpanzees | journal=Molecular Biology and Evolution | volume=21 | issue=5 | pages=799–808 | date=May 2004 | pmid=14963091 | doi=10.1093/molbev/msh083}}
13. ^{{cite journal |author=de Manuel, M. |display-authors=etal | title=Chimpanzee genomic diversity reveals ancient admixture with bonobos | journal=Science | volume=354 | issue=6311 | pages=477–481 | date=October 2016 | pmid=27789843 | pmc=5546212 | doi=10.1126/science.aag2602 | bibcode=2016Sci...354..477D}}
14. ^{{cite news |last1=Pickrell |first1=John |title=Chimps Belong on Human Branch of Family Tree, Study Says |url=http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2003/05/0520_030520_chimpanzees.html |access-date=11 June 2016 |work=National Geographic News |agency=National Geographic |date=20 May 2003}}
15. ^{{cite book |author=Groves, Colin P. | year=2001 | title=Primate Taxonomy | pages=303–307 | location=Washington, DC | publisher=Smithsonian Institution Press | isbn=9781560988724}}
16. ^{{cite book |author1=Hof, J. |author2=Sommer, V. | title=Apes Like Us: Portraits of a Kinship | publisher=Panorama | location=Mannheim | date=2010 | isbn=978-3-89823-435-1 | page=114}}
17. ^{{cite journal |author=Groves, Colin P. |title=Geographic variation within eastern chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes cf. schweinfurthii Giglioli, 1872) |journal=Australasian Primatology |year=2005}}
18. ^{{cite journal | author=Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium | title=Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome | journal=Nature | volume=437 | issue=7055 | pages=69–87 | date=September 2005 | pmid=16136131 | doi=10.1038/nature04072 | bibcode=2005Natur.437...69.}}
{{cite journal |author=Cheng, Z. |display-authors=etal | title=A genome-wide comparison of recent chimpanzee and human segmental duplications | journal=Nature | volume=437 | issue=7055 | pages=88–93 | date=September 2005 | pmid=16136132 | doi=10.1038/nature04000 | bibcode=2005Natur.437...88C}}
19. ^{{cite journal |author1=Towle, I. |author2=Irish, J. D. |author3=De Groote, I. | title=Amelogenesis imperfecta in the dentition of a wild chimpanzee | journal=Journal of Medical Primatology | volume=47 | issue=2 | pages=117–119 | date=April 2018 | pmid=29112236 | doi=10.1111/jmp.12323 | url=http://researchonline.ljmu.ac.uk/7572/1/Towle%20et%20al%20Submitted%20Amelogenesis%20imperfecta%20JMP.pdf}}
20. ^{{cite web |author=Cawthon Lang, K. | date=13 April 2006 | title=Primate Factsheets: Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) | url=http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/factsheets/entry/chimpanzee | access-date=29 January 2012}}
21. ^{{cite news | url=http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,495787,00.html | title=Chimpanzee Attack Revives Calls for Federal Primate Law | date=18 February 2009 | publisher=Fox News | access-date=28 January 2012}}
22. ^Eskeletons. Eskeletons.org Retrieved on 2013-04-18.
23. ^{{cite book |editor1=Burnie, D. |editor2=Wilson, D. E. | title=Animal: The Definitive Visual Guide to the World's Wildlife | publisher=DK Adult | date=2005 | isbn=978-0789477644}}
24. ^WCMC Species sheets
25. ^{{cite web |title=Chimpanzee Fact File |author1=Janssen, E. |author2=Janssen, P. |url=http://www.outtoafrica.nl/animals/engchimpanzee.html |publisher=African Wildlife Foundation |year=2006 |access-date=23 September 2012}}
26. ^{{cite journal |author=Poulsen, J. R. |author2=Clark, C. J. | year=2004 | title=Densities, distributions, and seasonal movements of gorillas and chimpanzees in swamp forest in northern Congo | journal=International Journal of Primatology | volume=25 | issue=2 | pages=285–306 | doi=10.1023/B:IJOP.0000019153.50161.58}}
27. ^{{cite journal |author=Sugiyama, Y. |author2=Koman, J. | year=1987 | title=A preliminary list of chimpanzees' alimentation at Bossou, Guinea | journal=Primates | volume=28 | issue=1 | pages=133–47 | doi=10.1007/BF02382192}}
28. ^Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H. (2000) The chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: behavioral ecology and evolution. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
29. ^{{cite web |last=Guernsey |first=Paul |title=What do chimps eat? |url=http://www.allaboutwildlife.com/what-do-chimps-eat |work=All About Wildlife |access-date=22 April 2013 |date=4 July 2009}}
30. ^{{cite book |author=Isabirye-Basuta, G. | date=1989 | chapter=Feeding ecology of chimpanzees in the Kibale Forest, Uganda | pages=116–127 |editor=Heltne, P. G. |editor2=Marquardt, L. A. | title=Understanding chimpanzees | location=Cambridge, Massachusetts | publisher=Harvard University Press | isbn=978-0674920910}}
31. ^{{cite book |author=Boesch, C. |author2=Uehara, S. |author3=Ihobe, H. | date=2002 | chapter=Variations in chimpanzee-red colobus interactions | pages=221–30 |editor=Boesch, C. |editor2=Hohmann, G. |editor3=Marchant, L. F. | title=Behavioral diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos | location=Cambridge, England | publisher=Cambridge University Press | isbn=978-0521006132}}
32. ^{{cite journal |author=Goodall, Jane |authorlink=Jane Goodall | title=Infant killing and cannibalism in free-living chimpanzees | journal=Folia Primatologica; International Journal of Primatology | volume=28 | issue=4 | pages=259–89 | year=1977 | pmid=564321 | doi=10.1159/000155817}}
33. ^{{cite web | url=http://www.allaboutwildlife.com/what-do-chimps-eat | title=What do Chimps Eat? | date=4 July 2009}}
34. ^{{cite web |last=Stanford |first=Craig |title=The Predatory Behavior and Ecology of Wild Chimpanzees |url=http://www-bcf.usc.edu/~stanford/chimphunt.html |publisher=USC |access-date=11 September 2013}}
35. ^{{cite journal |title=Chimpanzee Hunting Behavior | journal=American Scientist | volume=83 | issue=3 | pages=256 | first=Nicholas E. | last=Newton-Fisher |url=https://kar.kent.ac.uk/27815/1/Newton-Fisher_2007_hunting_review.PDF | bibcode=1995AmSci..83..256S | year=1995}}
36. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.bbc.co.uk/nature/species/Common_Chimpanzee#p004hd8g |title=Chimps on the hunt |publisher=BBC Wildlife Finder |date=24 October 1990 |access-date=2009-09-22}}
37. ^{{cite journal |last1=Van Lawick-Goodall |first1=Jane |authorlink=Jane Goodall |year=1968 |title=The Behaviour of Free-Living Chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve |journal=Animal Behaviour Monographs |volume=1 |issue=3 |page=191}}
38. ^{{cite web |author=Mulchay, J. B. |title=How long do chimpanzees live? |publisher=Chimpanzee Sanctuary Northwest|url=https://chimpsnw.org/2013/03/how-long-do-chimpanzees-live/|accessdate=2019-03-28}}
39. ^{{cite journal |author=Henschel, P. |author2=Abernethy, K. A. |author3=White, L. J. | year=2005 | title=Leopard food habits in the Lopé National Park, Gabon, Central Africa | journal=African Journal of Ecology | volume=43 | issue=1 | pages=21–8 | doi=10.1111/j.1365-2028.2004.00518.x}}
40. ^{{cite journal |author=Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. |display-authors=etal | title=Aggression toward large carnivores by wild chimpanzees of Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania | journal=Folia Primatologica; International Journal of Primatology | volume=47 | issue=1 | pages=8–13 | date=11 September 2008 | pmid=3557232 | doi=10.1159/000156259}}
41. ^{{cite journal | author=Tsukahara, T. |title=Lions eat chimpanzees: The first evidence of predation by lions on wild chimpanzees |journal=American Journal of Primatology |date=10 September 1992 |volume=29 |issue=1 |pages=1–11 |doi=10.1002/ajp.1350290102}}
42. ^{{cite journal |author=Weiss, Robin A. |year=2009 |title=Apes, lice and prehistory |journal=Journal of Biology |volume=8 |issue=2 |page=20 |doi=10.1186/jbiol114 |pmc=2687769 |pmid=19232074}}
43. ^{{cite journal |author=McLennan, Matthew R. |author2=Hasegawa, Hideo |author3=Bardi, Massimo |author4=Huffman, Michael A. |year=2017 |title=Gastrointestinal parasite infections and self-medication in wild chimpanzees surviving in degraded forest fragments within an agricultural landscape mosaic in Uganda |journal=PLoS One |volume=12 |issue=7 |at=e0180431 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0180431}}
44. ^[https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/science-environment-40348890 Watched chimps change their hunting habits]
45. ^{{cite journal |author=Van Lawick-Goodall, Jane |authorlink=Jane Goodall |year=1968 |title=The Behaviour of Free-Living Chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve |journal=Animal Behaviour Monographs (Rutgers University) |volume=1 |issue=3 |page=167}}
46. ^The Chimpanzees of Tanzania. Wild Kingdom. 31 December 1976.
47. ^{{cite journal |author=Goldberg, T. L. |author2=Wrangham, R. W. | title=Genetic correlates of social behaviour in wild chimpanzees: evidence from mitochondrial DNA | journal=Animal Behaviour | volume=54 | issue=3 | pages=559–70 | date=September 1997 | pmid=9299041 | doi=10.1006/anbe.1996.0450}}
48. ^10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 {{cite book | last=Goodall | first=Jane | authorlink=Jane Goodall | year=1986 | title=The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior | isbn=978-0-674-11649-8}}
49. ^{{cite journal |author=Pepper, J. W. |author2=Mitani, J. C. |author3=Watts, D. P. | year=1999 | title=General gregariousness and specific social preferences among wild chimpanzees | journal=International Journal of Primatology | volume=20 | issue=5 | pages=613–32 | doi=10.1023/A:1020760616641 | citeseerx=10.1.1.1000.4734 }}
50. ^{{cite book |author=Muller, M. N. | date=2002 | chapter=Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees | pages=112–124 |editor=Boesch, C. |display-editors=etal | title=Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos | location=Cambridge | publisher=Cambridge University Press | isbn=0521006139}}
51. ^{{cite book |author=Bygott, J. D. | date=1979 | chapter=Agonistic behavior, dominance, and social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park | pages=73–121 |editor=Hamburg, D. A. |editor2=McCown E. R. | title=The great apes | location=Menlo Park | publisher=Benjamin-Cummings | isbn=978-0805336696}}
52. ^{{cite book |author=de Waal, F. B. | authorlink=Frans de Waal |date=1987 | chapter=Dynamic of social relationships | pages=421–429 |editor=Smuts, B. B. |display-editors=etal | title=Primate societies | location=Chicago | publisher=University of Chicago Press | isbn=978-0226767161}}
53. ^{{cite book |author=Nishida, T. |author2=Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. | date=1986 | chapter=Chimpanzees and Bonobos: Cooperative Relationships among Males | pages=165–177 |editor=Smuts, B. B. |display-editors=etal | title=Primate Societies | location=Chicago and London | publisher=The University of Chicago Press | isbn=978-0226767161}}
54. ^{{cite book |author=Stumpf, R. | date=2007 | chapter=Chimpanzees and bonobos: Diversity within and between species | pages=321–344 |editor=Campbell C. J. |display-editors=etal | title=Primates in perspective | location=New York | publisher=Oxford University Press | isbn=978-0195390438}}
55. ^{{cite journal |author=Watts, D. P. | year=2001 | title=Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees |journal=Behaviour | volume=139 | issue=2 | pages=343–370 | doi=10.1163/156853902760102708 | url=http://www2.unine.ch/repository/default/content/sites/ethol/files/shared/documents/watts_2002.pdf | citeseerx=10.1.1.516.3624}}
56. ^{{cite news |last=Walsh |first=Bryan |url=http://www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,1880229,00.html |title=Why the Stamford Chimp Attacked |publisher=TIME |date=18 February 2009 |access-date=2009-06-06}}
57. ^{{cite news |url=http://www.economist.com/node/16422404 |title=Killer Instincts |publisher=The Economist |date=24 June 2010}}
58. ^{{cite book |author=Wallis, J. | date=2002 | chapter=Seasonal aspects of reproduction and sexual behavior in two chimpanzee populations: a comparison of Gombe (Tanzania) and Budongo (Uganda) | pages=181–191 |editor=Boesch, C. |editor2=Hohmann, G. |editor3=Marchant, L. F. | title=Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos | location=Cambridge (England) | publisher=Cambridge University Press | isbn=978-0521006132}}
59. ^{{cite journal |author=Gagneux, P. |author2=Boesch, C. |author3=Woodruff, D. S. | title=Female reproductive strategies, paternity and community structure in wild West African chimpanzees | journal=Animal Behaviour | volume=57 | issue=1 | pages=19–32 | date=January 1999 | pmid=10053068 | doi=10.1006/anbe.1998.0972}}
60. ^{{cite journal | author1=Pusey, A. |author2=Williams, J. |author3=Goodall, J. |author3link=Jane Goodall | title=The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees | journal=Science | volume=277 | issue=5327 | pages=828–831 | date=August 1997 | pmid=9242614 | doi=10.1126/science.277.5327.828}}
61. ^{{cite book |author=Bard, K. A. | date=1995 | chapter=Parenting in primates | pages=27–58 | editor=Bornstein, M. H. | title=Handbook of parenting | volume=Volume 2: Biology and ecology of parenting | location=Mahwah, New Jersey | publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates | isbn=978-0805837797}}
62. ^{{Cite journal |last=Fedurek |first=Pawel |last2=Zuberbühler |first2=Klaus |last3=Semple |first3=Stuart |date=2017-11-06 |title=Trade-offs in the production of animal vocal sequences: insights from the structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots |journal=Frontiers in Zoology |volume=14 |pages=50 |doi=10.1186/s12983-017-0235-8 |pmc=5674848 |pmid=29142585}}
63. ^{{cite journal |author=Crockford, C. |author2=Boesch, C. | year=2005 | title=Call combinations in wild chimpanzees | journal=Behaviour | volume=142 | issue=4 | pages=397–421 | doi=10.1163/1568539054012047}}
64. ^{{cite journal |author=Boesch, C. | title=Cooperative hunting roles among taï chimpanzees | journal=Human Nature | volume=13 | issue=1 | pages=27–46 | date=March 2002 | pmid=26192594 | doi=10.1007/s12110-002-1013-6 | citeseerx=10.1.1.556.2265}}
65. ^{{cite book |author=Boesch, C. |author2=Boesch, H. | date=1993 | chapter=Diversity of tool use and tool-making in wild chimpanzees | pages=158–87 |editor=Berthelet, A. |editor2=Chavaillon, J. | title=The use of tools by human and non-human primates | location=Oxford, England | publisher=Oxford University Press | isbn=978-0198522638}}
66. ^{{cite journal |author=Mercader. J. |display-authors=etal | title=4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology | journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume=104 | issue=9 | pages=3043–8 | date=February 2007 | pmid=17360606 | pmc=1805589 | doi=10.1073/pnas.0607909104 | bibcode=2007PNAS..104.3043M}}
67. ^{{cite book |title=The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior | author=Goodall, J. | authorlink=Jane Goodall |pages=535–539 |year=1986 |publisher=The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press |isbn=978-0-674-11649-8}}
68. ^{{cite book |title=In the Shadow of Man | author=Goodall, J. | authorlink=Jane Goodall |pages=35–37 |year=1971 |publisher=Houghton Mifflin |isbn=978-0-395-33145-3}}
69. ^{{cite web |title=Gombe Timeline |url=http://www.janegoodall.org/jane/study-corner/chimpanzees/gombe-timeline.asp |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080125194313/http://www.janegoodall.org/jane/study-corner/chimpanzees/gombe-timeline.asp |archive-date=25 January 2008 |publisher=Jane Goodall Institute |access-date=2009-03-05}}
70. ^{{cite journal |author=Stanford, C. B. |display-authors=etal | title=Chimpanzees in Bwindi-Impenetrable National Park, Uganda, use different tools to obtain different types of honey | journal=Primates; Journal of Primatology | volume=41 | issue=3 | pages=337–341 | date=July 2000 | pmid=30545184 | doi=10.1007/BF02557602}}
71. ^{{cite journal | author=Sugiyama, Y. | year=1995 | title=Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou | journal=American Journal of Primatology | volume=37 | issue=1 | pages=263–269 | doi=10.1002/ajp.1350370308}}
72. ^{{cite web |url=http://news.discovery.com/animals/female-chimps-seen-making-wielding-spears-150414.htm |title=Female Chimps Seen Making, Wielding Spears |last1=Viegas |first1=Jennifer |date=14 April 2015 |publisher=Discovery |access-date=15 April 2015}}
73. ^{{cite journal |author=Huffman, M. A. |author2=Kalunde, M. S. |title=Tool-assisted predation on a squirrel by a female chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania |journal=Primates |date=January 1993 |volume=34 |issue=1 |pages=93–98 |doi=10.1007/BF02381285}}
74. ^The study was presented in a Channel 5 (UK) documentary "The Memory Chimp", part of the channel's Extraordinary Animals series.
75. ^{{cite journal |last1=Melis | first1=Alicia P. |first2=Brian |last2=Hare |first3=Michael |last3=Tomasello |title=Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators |journal=Science |volume=311 |issue=5765 |year=2006b |pages=1297–1300 |ref=harv |bibcode=2006Sci...311.1297M |doi=10.1126/science.1123007 | pmid=16513985}}
76. ^{{cite journal |last1=Suchak |first1=Malini |first2=Timothy M. |last2=Eppley |first3=Matthew W. |last3=Campbell |first4=Rebecca A. |last4=Feldman |first5=Luke F. |last5=Quarles |first6=Frans B. M. |last6=de Waal |author6link=Frans de Waal |title=How chimpanzees cooperate in a competitive world |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |year=2016 |volume=113 |issue=36 |pages=10215–10220 | doi=10.1073/pnas.1611826113 |pmid=27551075 |ref=harv |pmc=5018789}}
77. ^{{Cite journal |author=Johnson, Steven | date=1 April 2003 |title=Emotions and the Brain |journal=Discover Magazine |url=http://discovermagazine.com/2003/apr/featlaugh}}
78. ^{{Cite journal |author1=Gardner, R. A. |author2=Gardner, B. T. |year=1969 |title=Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee |journal=Science |volume=165 |pages=664–672 |doi=10.1126/science.165.3894.664 |pmid=5793972 |issue=3894 |bibcode=1969Sci...165..664G |citeseerx=10.1.1.384.4164 }}
79. ^{{Cite book |author1=Allen, G. R. |author2=Gardner, B. T. |year=1980 |chapter=Comparative psychology and language acquisition |editor=Thomas A. Sebok and Jean-Umiker-Sebok |title=Speaking of Apes: A Critical Anthology of Two-Way Communication with Man |location=New York |publisher=Plenum Press |pages=287–329 |isbn=978-0306402791}}
80. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.greatapetrust.org/bonobo/language/ |title=Language of Bonobos |publisher=Great Ape Trust |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20040815161414/http://www.greatapetrust.org/bonobo/language/ |archivedate=August 15, 2004 |accessdate=2012-01-16}}
81. ^{{cite web |last=Wynne |first=Clive |url=http://www.skeptic.com/eskeptic/07-10-31 |title=eSkeptic |publisher=Skeptic |date=October 31, 2007 |accessdate=2011-01-28}}
82. ^{{cite book | first=Hope, B. | last=Werness |year=2007 |title=The Continuum Encyclopedia of Animal Symbolism in World Art |publisher=Continuum International Publishing Group |page=86 |isbn=978-0826419132}}
83. ^{{Cite book |last=Van Riper |first=A. Bowdoin |title=Science in popular culture: a reference guide |publisher=Greenwood Press |location=Westport |year=2002 |pages=18–19 |isbn=978-0-313-31822-1}}
84. ^{{cite book |last=Warner |first=Marina |authorlink=Marina Warner |title=Monsters of our own making: the peculiar pleasures of fear |publisher=University Press of Kentucky |year=2007 |page=335 |isbn=978-0813191744}}
85. ^{{cite web |last1=Heath |first1=Neil |title=PG Tips chimps: The last of the tea-advertising apes |url=https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-england-leicestershire-23508215 |publisher=BBC |accessdate=30 March 2019 |date=9 January 2014}}
86. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.nomoremonkeybusiness.com/ |title=Animal Actors |publisher=Nomoremonkeybusiness.com |accessdate=2011-01-28}}
87. ^{{Cite news | url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/7839819.stm | work=BBC News | title=Gorilla diary: August – December 2008 | date=20 January 2009 |accessdate=2010-04-28}}
88. ^{{cite web |title=Chimpanzees Don't Make Good Pets |url=http://www.janegoodall.org/chimp-central-pets |website=The Jane Goodall Institute |accessdate=1 February 2015 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20150202195221/http://www.janegoodall.org/chimp-central-pets |archivedate=2 February 2015}}
89. ^{{cite web |url=http://www.hsus.org/animals_in_research/chimps_deserve_better/research/chimpanzee-lab-and-sanctuary-map.html |title=Chimpanzee lab and sanctuary map |publisher=The Humane Society of the United States |accessdate=2008-03-24 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20080307055400/http://www.hsus.org/animals_in_research/chimps_deserve_better/research/chimpanzee-lab-and-sanctuary-map.html |archivedate=March 7, 2008 }}
90. ^{{cite web | url=http://www.hsus.org/animals_in_research/chimps_deserve_better/research/overview_of_research_uses_and.html | title=Chimpanzee Research: Overview of Research Uses and Costs | publisher=Humane Society of the United States | accessdate=2008-03-24 |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20080307055406/http://www.hsus.org/animals_in_research/chimps_deserve_better/research/overview_of_research_uses_and.html |archivedate=March 7, 2008}}
91. ^Lovgren, Stefan. Should Labs Treat Chimps More Like Humans?, National Geographic News, September 6, 2005.
92. ^[https://web.archive.org/web/20080215112131/http://www.hsus.org/animals_in_research/chimps_deserve_better/ Chimps Deserve Better], Humane Society of the United States.
93. ^Langley, Gill (June 2006). Next of Kin: A Report on the Use of Primates in Experiments, British Union for the Abolition of Vivisection, p. 15, citing {{cite journal |vauthors=VandeBerg JL, Zola SM |title=A unique biomedical resource at risk |journal=Nature |volume=437 |issue=7055 |pages=30–2 |date=September 2005 |pmid=16136112 |doi=10.1038/437030a |bibcode=2005Natur.437...30V }}
94. ^{{cite journal |doi=10.1080/15299730802073619 |vauthors=Bradshaw GA, Capaldo T, Lindner L, Grow G |title=Building an inner sanctuary: complex PTSD in chimpanzees |journal=J Trauma Dissociation |volume=9 |issue=1 |pages=9–34 |year=2008 |pmid=19042307 |url=http://www.releasechimps.org/pdfs/ExecSumTraumaFINAL.pdf}}
95. ^{{cite news |last=Cohen |first=Joel E. |title=Going Bananas |publisher=American Scholar |date=Winter 1993 |pages=154–157}}
96. ^{{cite journal | last=Power | first=Margaret |title=Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees |journal=American Anthropologist |volume=95 |date=December 1993 |pages=1010–11 |issue=4 | doi=10.1525/aa.1993.95.4.02a00180}}
97. ^{{cite web | url=http://www.monkeyday.org/2006/04/texas-man-tells-story-of-fatal-chimp.html | title=Texas man saves friend during fatal chimp attack | access-date=2006-06-27 | last=Osborn | first=Claire |date=27 April 2006 | work=The Pulse Journal}}
98. ^{{cite news | title=Chimp attack kills cabbie and injures tourists | url=https://www.theguardian.com/international/story/0,,1760554,00.html | publisher=The Guardian | access-date=2006-06-27 | date=25 April 2006 | location=London}}
99. ^{{cite web | url=http://www.primates.com/chimps/drunk-n-disorderly.html | title='Drunk and Disorderly' Chimps Attacking Ugandan Children | access-date=2006-06-27 | date=9 February 2004}}
100. ^{{cite web | url=http://virus.stanford.edu/filo/eboci.html | title =Ebola Cote D'Ivoire Outbreaks | publisher=Stanford University | year=1999 | last=Waterman | first=Tara | access-date=2008-03-24}}
101. ^{{cite web | url=http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/7087194/ | title=Chimp attack doesn't surprise experts | access-date=2006-06-27 | date=5 March 2005 | work=MSNBC}}
102. ^{{cite web |url=http://ngm.nationalgeographic.com/ngm/0304/feature4/online_extra2.html |title=Online Extra: Frodo @ National Geographic Magazine |publisher=Ngm.nationalgeographic.com |date=15 May 2002 |access-date=2009-06-06}}
103. ^{{cite news | url=https://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2005-03-04-chimp-attack_x.htm | title=Birthday party turns bloody when chimps attack | access-date=2006-06-27 | date=4 March 2005 | work =USATODAY}}
104. ^{{cite news | title=The Animal Within | access-date=2006-06-27 | url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2005/05/23/AR2005052301819.html | date=24 May 2005 | last=Argetsinger | first=Amy | work=The Washington Post}}
105. ^{{cite news |url=http://www.nydailynews.com/news/2009/02/17/2009-02-17_911_tape_captures_chimpanzee_owners_horr-2.html |title=911 tape captures chimpanzee owner's horror as 200-pound ape mauls friend |publisher=Nydailynews.com |date=18 February 2009 |access-date=2009-06-06 |location=New York |first1=Edgar |last1=Sandoval |deadurl=yes |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090219200257/http://www.nydailynews.com/news/2009/02/17/2009-02-17_911_tape_captures_chimpanzee_owners_horr-2.html |archive-date=19 February 2009}}
106. ^{{cite news | last=Gallman | first=Stephanie |url=http://edition.cnn.com/2009/US/02/17/chimpanzee.attack/ |title=Chimp attack 911 call: 'He's ripping her apart' |publisher=CNN |date=18 February 2009 |access-date=2009-06-06}}
107. ^{{cite journal |author=Reeves, J. D. |author2=Doms, R. W. | title=Human immunodeficiency virus type 2 | journal=The Journal of General Virology | volume=83 | issue=Pt 6 | pages=1253–65 | date=June 2002 | pmid=12029140 | doi=10.1099/0022-1317-83-6-1253 | url=http://vir.sgmjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=12029140 | archive-url=https://archive.today/20121228095942/http://vir.sgmjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=12029140 | archive-date=28 December 2012 | dead-url=yes | df=dmy-all | citeseerx=10.1.1.523.5120 }}
108. ^{{cite journal |last1=Keele |first1= B. F. |display-authors= etal | title=Chimpanzee reservoirs of pandemic and nonpandemic HIV-1 | journal=Science | volume=313 | issue=5786 | pages=523–526 | date=July 2006 | pmid=16728595 | pmc=2442710 | doi=10.1126/science.1126531 | bibcode=2006Sci...313..523K}}
109. ^{{cite journal |author=Gao, F. |display-authors=etal | title=Origin of HIV-1 in the chimpanzee Pan troglodytes troglodytes | journal=Nature | volume=397 | issue=6718 | pages=436–41 | date=February 1999 | pmid=9989410 | doi=10.1038/17130 | bibcode=1999Natur.397..436G}}
110. ^{{cite news |last1=St.Fleur |first1=Nicholas |title=U.S. Will Call All Chimps ‘Endangered’ |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2015/06/13/science/chimpanzees-endangered-fish-and-wildlife-service.html?action=click&pgtype=Homepage&version=Moth-Hidden&module=inside-nyt-region®ion=inside-nyt-region&WT.nav=inside-nyt-region |access-date=13 June 2015 |work=The New York Times |date=12 June 2015}}
111. ^{{cite web |title=U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service Finalizes Rule Listing All Chimpanzees as Endangered Under the Endangered Species Act |url=https://www.fws.gov/news/ShowNews.cfm?ID=E81DA137-BAF2-9619-3492A2972E9854D9 |website=fws.gov |publisher=U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service |access-date=13 June 2015}}

External links

{{Commons category |Pan_troglodytes |Common chimpanzee}}{{Wikispecies |Pan troglodytes |Common Chimpanzee}}
  • DiscoverChimpanzees.org
  • [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/projects/genome/guide/chimp/ Chimpanzee Genome resources]
  • Primate Info Net Pan troglodytes Factsheets
  • U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service Species Profile
  • View the common chimpanzee genome in Ensembl
  • Human Timeline (Interactive) – Smithsonian, National Museum of Natural History (August 2016).
{{Hominidae nav}}{{Apes}}{{Taxonbar |from=Q4126704}}{{Authority control}}{{DEFAULTSORT:chimpanzee, common}}

6 : Chimpanzees|Tool-using mammals|Primates of Africa|Extant Pliocene first appearances|Mammals described in 1776|Articles containing video clips

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/9/22 18:24:20