词条 | Grandmother cell |
释义 |
The grandmother cell, sometimes called the "Jennifer Aniston neuron," is a hypothetical neuron that represents a complex but specific concept or object.[1] It activates when a person "sees, hears, or otherwise sensibly discriminates"[2] a specific entity, such as his or her grandmother. The term was in use at least as early as 1966 amongst staff and students in the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, England. A similar concept, that of the gnostic neuron, was proposed two years later by Jerzy Konorski.[3] SupportFace selective cellsVisual neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the monkey fire selectively to hands and faces.[4][5][6][7] These cells are selective in that they do not fire for other visual objects important for monkeys such as fruit and genitalia. Research finds that some of these cells can be trained to show high specificity for arbitrary visual objects, and these would seem to fit the requirements of gnostic/grandmother cells.[8][9] In addition, evidence exists for cells in the human hippocampus that have highly selective responses to gnostic categories[10][11] including highly selective responses to individual human faces.[12] However most of the reported face-selective cells are not grandmother/gnostic cells since they do not represent a specific percept, that is, they are not cells narrowly selective in their activations for one face and only one face irrespective of transformations of size, orientation, and color. Even the most selective face cells usually also discharge, if more weakly, to a variety of individual faces. Furthermore, face-selective cells often vary in their responsiveness to different aspects of faces. This suggests that cell responsiveness arises from the need of a monkey to differentiate among different individual faces rather than among other categories of stimuli such as bananas with their discrimination properties linked to the fact that different individual faces are much more similar to each other in their overall organization and fine detail than other kinds of stimuli.[1] Moreover, it has been suggested that these cells might in fact be responding as specialized feature detector neurons that only function in the holistic context of a face construct.[13][14] One idea has been that such cells form ensembles for the coarse or distributed coding of faces rather than detectors for specific faces. Thus, a specific grandmother may be represented by a specialized ensemble of grandmother or near grandmother cells.[1] Individual specific recognition cellsIn 2005, a UCLA and Caltech study found evidence of different cells that fire in response to particular people, such as Bill Clinton or Jennifer Aniston. A neuron for Halle Berry, for example, might respond "to the concept, the abstract entity, of Halle Berry", and would fire not only for images of Halle Berry, but also to the actual name "Halle Berry".[15] However, there is no suggestion in that study that only the cell being monitored responded to that concept, nor was it suggested that no other actress would cause that cell to respond (although several other presented images of actresses did not cause it to respond).[15] The researchers believe that they have found evidence for sparseness, rather than for grandmother cells.[16] Further evidence for the theory that a small neural network provides facial recognition was found from analysis of cell recording studies of macaque monkeys. By formatting faces as points in a high-dimensional linear space, the scientists discovered that each face cell’s firing rate is proportional to the projection of an incoming face stimulus onto a single axis in this space, allowing a face cell ensemble of about 200 cells to encode the location of any face in the space.[17] Sparseness vs distributed representationsThe grandmother cell hypothesis, is an extreme version of the idea of sparseness,[18][19] and is not without critics. The opposite of the grandmother cell theory is the distributed representation theory, that states that a specific stimulus is coded by its unique pattern of activity over a large group of neurons widely distributed in the brain. The arguments against the sparseness include:
Pontifical cellsWilliam James in 1890 proposed a related idea of a pontifical cell.[20] The pontifical cell is defined as a putative, and implausible cell which had all our experiences. It is in this different from a concept specific cell in that it is the site of experience of sense data. James's 1890 pontifical cell was instead a cell "to which the rest of the brain provided a representation" of a grandmother. The experience of grandmother occurred in this cell. See also
References1. ^1 2 {{cite journal | last1 = Gross | first1 = CG | year = 2002 | title = "Genealogy of the "Grandmother Cell | url = http://www.as.wvu.edu/daly/439/readings/Gross%202002%20Grandmother%20Cell.pdf | journal = Neuroscientist | volume = 8 | issue = 5 | pages = 512–518 | doi = 10.1177/107385802237175 | pmid = 12374433 | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20080829143727/http://www.as.wvu.edu/daly/439/readings/Gross%202002%20Grandmother%20Cell.pdf | archivedate = 2008-08-29 | df = }} 2. ^{{cite book|last =Clark|first=Austen| title = A Theory of Sentience| year = 2000 | isbn = 978-0-19-823851-5 |publisher = Oxford University Press|page = 43}} 3. ^Konorski J. 1967. Integrative activity of the brain; an interdisciplinary approach.(1967) 4. ^Gross CG. 1998a. Brain, vision, memory: tales in the history of neuroscience. 5. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Perrett | first1 = DI | last2 = Rolls | first2 = ET | last3 = Caan | first3 = W | year = 1982 | title = Visual neurons responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. | url = | journal = Exp Brain Res | volume = 47 | issue = 3| pages = 329–42 | doi=10.1007/bf00239352| pmid = 7128705 }} 6. ^Rolls ET. 1984. Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Hum Neurobiol 3:209–22. 7. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Yamane | first1 = S | last2 = Kaji | first2 = S | last3 = Kawano | first3 = K | year = 1988 | title = What facial features activate face neurons in the inferotemporal cortex of the monkey? | url = | journal = Exp Brain Res | volume = 73 | issue = | pages = 209–14 | doi=10.1007/bf00279674}} 8. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Logothetis | first1 = NK | last2 = Sheinberg | first2 = DL | year = 1996 | title = Visual object recognition. | url = | journal = Annu Rev Neurosci | volume = 19 | issue = | pages = 577–621 | doi=10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.003045| pmid = 8833455 }} 9. ^Tanaka K. 1996. Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 19:109–39. 10. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Gross | first1 = CG | year = 2000 | title = Coding for visual categories in the human brain | url = | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 3 | issue = 9| pages = 855–6 | doi=10.1038/78745| pmid = 10966610 }} 11. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Kreiman | first1 = G | last2 = Koch | first2 = C | last3 = Fried | first3 = I | year = 2000 | title = Category specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe | doi = 10.1038/78868 | pmid = 10966627 | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 3 | issue = 9| pages = 946–53 }} 12. ^Kreiman G, Fried I, Koch C. 2001. Single neuron responses in humans during binocular rivalry and flash suppression. Abstr Soc Neurosci 27 13. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Jagadeesh | first1 = B | year = 2009 | title = Recognizing grandmother | url = | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 12 | issue = 9| pages = 1083–5 | doi = 10.1038/nn0909-1083 | pmid = 19710647 }} 14. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Freiwald | first1 = WA | last2 = Tsao | first2 = DY | last3 = Livingstone | first3 = MS | year = 2009 | title = A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 12 | issue = 9| pages = 1187–96 | doi = 10.1038/nn.2363 | pmid = 19668199 | pmc=2819705}} 15. ^1 {{cite journal | last1 = Quiroga | first1 = R. | display-authors = etal | year = 2005 | title = Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain | url = | journal = Nature | volume = 435 | issue = 7045| pages = 1102–1107 | doi = 10.1038/nature03687 | pmid = 15973409 }} 16. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Quiroga | first1 = R. | display-authors = etal | year = 2008 | title = Sparse but not 'Grandmother-cell' coding in the medial temporal lobe | url = | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 12 | issue = 3| pages = 87–91| doi = 10.1016/j.tics.2007.12.003 | pmid = 18262826 }} 17. ^{{Cite journal|last=Chang, Tsao|date=2017|title=The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain|journal=Cell|volume=169|issue=6|pages=1013–1028|doi=10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011}} 18. ^{{cite book | last = Rugg | first = Michael | title = Cognitive Neuroscience | publisher = MIT Press | year = 1997 | isbn = 978-0-262-68094-3 | pages=49–58 | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Cpo_2d7tixUC&pg=PA49&vq=spatial+aspects+of+coding&dq=sparseness+neuroscience}} 19. ^{{Cite journal | last = Connor | first = Charles | title = Friends and grandmothers | journal = Nature | volume = 435 | pages = 1036–1037 | date = 23 June 2005 | doi = 10.1038/4351036a | pmid = 15973389 | issue = 7045 }} 20. ^James W. (1890). The principles of psychology.New York: Dover 2 : Neural coding|Cognitive science |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。