请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Time perception
释义

  1. Theories

  2. Philosophical perspectives

  3. Neuroscientific perspectives

  4. Types of temporal illusions

      Kappa effect    Eye movements and "Chronostasis"   Oddball effect   Effects of emotional states   Awe  Fear  Empathy  Depression   Changes with age    Effects of drugs    Effects of body temperature    Reversal of temporal order judgment   Flash-lag effect   Effects of clinical disorders   Sleep 

  5. See also

  6. References

  7. Further reading

  8. External links

{{merge from|Tachypsychia|discuss=Talk:Time perception#Merge from Tachypsychia|date=July 2018}}

Time perception is a field of study within psychology, cognitive linguistics[1] and neuroscience that refers to the subjective experience, or sense, of time, which is measured by someone's own perception of the duration of the indefinite and unfolding of events.[2][3] The perceived time interval between two successive events is referred to as perceived duration. Though directly experiencing or understanding another person's perception of time is not possible, such a perception can be objectively studied and inferred through a number of scientific experiments. Time perception is a construction of the sapient brain, but one that is manipulable and distortable under certain circumstances. These temporal illusions help to expose the underlying neural mechanisms of time perception.

Pioneering work, emphasizing species-specific differences, was conducted by Karl Ernst von Baer.[4]

In other words time can be perceived or understood as Subjective Time & Objective Time.

Theories

William J. Friedman (1993) also contrasted two theories for a sense of time:[5][5][6]
  • The strength model of time memory. This posits a memory trace that persists over time, by which one might judge the age of a memory (and therefore how long ago the event remembered occurred) from the strength of the trace. This conflicts with the fact that memories of recent events may fade more quickly than more distant memories.
  • The inference model suggests the time of an event is inferred from information about relations between the event in question and other events whose date or time is known.

Another theory involves the brain's subconscious tallying of "pulses" during a specific interval, forming a biological stopwatch. This theory alleges that the brain can run multiple biological stopwatches at one time depending on the type of task one is involved in. The location of these pulses and what these pulses actually consist of is unclear.[7] This model is only a metaphor and does not stand up in terms of brain physiology or anatomy.[9]

Philosophical perspectives

{{main|Specious present}}

The specious present is the time duration wherein a state of consciousness is experienced as being in the present.[10] The term was first introduced by the philosopher E. R. Clay in 1882 (E. Robert Kelly),[11] and was further developed by William James. James defined the specious present to be "the prototype of all conceived times... the short duration of which we are immediately and incessantly sensible". In "Scientific Thought" (1930), C. D. Broad further elaborated on the concept of the specious present and considered that the specious present may be considered as the temporal equivalent of a sensory datum. A version of the concept was used by Edmund Husserl in his works and discussed further by Francisco Varela based on the writings of Husserl, Heidegger, and Merleau-Ponty.[15] Although he lived prior to these modern philosophers, Hasidic master Rabbi Nachman of Breslov (1772-1810) discussed the nature and perception of time in various teachings. He remarked that only the present day and present moment are "real," [8] and also noted that a person could sleep for fifteen minutes and dream that he or she had lived seventy years [9]

Neuroscientific perspectives

Although the perception of time is not associated with a specific sensory system, psychologists and neuroscientists suggest that humans do have a system, or several complementary systems, governing the perception of time.[10] Time perception is handled by a highly distributed system involving the cerebral cortex, cerebellum and basal ganglia.[19] One particular component, the suprachiasmatic nucleus, is responsible for the circadian (or daily) rhythm, while other cell clusters appear to be capable of shorter (ultradian) timekeeping. There is some evidence that very short (millisecond) durations are processed by dedicated neurons in early sensory parts of the brain[11][12]

Professor Warren Meck devised a physiological model for measuring the passage of time. He found the representation of time to be generated by the oscillatory activity of cells in the upper cortex. The frequency of these cells' activity is detected by cells in the dorsal striatum at the base of the forebrain. His model separated explicit timing and implicit timing. Explicit timing is used in estimating the duration of a stimulus. Implicit timing is used to gauge the amount of time separating one from an impending event that is expected to occur in the near future. These two estimations of time do not involve the same neuroanatomical areas. For example, implicit timing often occurs to achieve a motor task, involving the cerebellum, left parietal cortex, and left premotor cortex. Explicit timing often involves the supplementary motor area and the right prefrontal cortex.[9]

Two visual stimuli, inside someone's field of view, can be successfully regarded as simultaneous up to five milliseconds.[13][14][15]

In the popular essay "Brain Time", David Eagleman explains that different types of sensory information (auditory, tactile, visual, etc.) are processed at different speeds by different neural architectures. The brain must learn how to overcome these speed disparities if it is to create a temporally unified representation of the external world: "if the visual brain wants to get events correct timewise, it may have only one choice: wait for the slowest information to arrive. To accomplish this, it must wait about a tenth of a second. In the early days of television broadcasting, engineers worried about the problem of keeping audio and video signals synchronized. Then they accidentally discovered that they had around a hundred milliseconds of slop: As long as the signals arrived within this window, viewers' brains would automatically resynchronize the signals". He goes on to say that "This brief waiting period allows the visual system to discount the various delays imposed by the early stages; however, it has the disadvantage of pushing perception into the past. There is a distinct survival advantage to operating as close to the present as possible; an animal does not want to live too far in the past. Therefore, the tenth-of- a-second window may be the smallest delay that allows higher areas of the brain to account for the delays created in the first stages of the system while still operating near the border of the present. This window of delay means that awareness is postdictive, incorporating data from a window of time after an event and delivering a retrospective interpretation of what happened."[16]

Experiments have shown that rats can successfully estimate a time interval of approximately 40 seconds, despite having their cortex entirely removed.[27] This suggests that time estimation may be a low level process.[28]

Types of temporal illusions

{{see also|Tachypsychia}}

A temporal illusion is a distortion in the perception of time. Time perception refers to a variety of time-related tasks. For example:

  • estimating time intervals, e.g., "When did you last see your primary care physician?";
  • estimating time duration, e.g., "How long were you waiting at the doctor's office?"; and
  • judging the simultaneity of events (see below for examples).

Short list of types of temporal illusions:

  • Telescoping effect: People tend to recall recent events as occurring further back in time than they actually did (backward telescoping) and distant events as occurring more recently than they actually did (forward telescoping).[29]
  • Vierordt's law: Shorter intervals tend to be overestimated while longer intervals tend to be underestimated
  • Time intervals associated with more changes may be perceived as longer than intervals with fewer changes
  • Perceived temporal length of a given task may shorten with greater motivation
  • Perceived temporal length of a given task may stretch when broken up or interrupted
  • Auditory stimuli may appear to last longer than visual stimuli[17][18][19][20]
  • Time durations may appear longer with greater stimulus intensity (e.g., auditory loudness or pitch)
  • Simultaneity judgments can be manipulated by repeated exposure to non-simultaneous stimuli

Kappa effect

The Kappa effect or perceptual time dilation[21] is a form of temporal illusion verifiable by experiment,[35] wherein the temporal duration between a sequence of consecutive stimuli is thought to be relatively longer or shorter than its actual elapsed time, due to the spatial/auditory/tactile separation between each consecutive stimuli. The kappa effect can be displayed when considering a journey made in two parts that take an equal amount of time. Between these two parts, the journey that covers more distance may appear to take longer than the journey covering less distance, even though they take an equal amount of time.

Eye movements and "Chronostasis"

The perception of space and time undergoes distortions during rapid saccadic eye movements[22]

Chronostasis is a type of temporal illusion in which the first impression following the introduction of a new event or task demand to the brain appears to be extended in time.[37] For example, chronostasis temporarily occurs when fixating on a target stimulus, immediately following a saccade (e.g., quick eye movement). This elicits an overestimation in the temporal duration for which that target stimulus (i.e., postsaccadic stimulus) was perceived. This effect can extend apparent durations by up to 500 ms and is consistent with the idea that the visual system models events prior to perception.[23] The most well-known version of this illusion is known as the stopped-clock illusion, wherein a subject's first impression of the second-hand movement of an analog clock, subsequent to one's directed attention (i.e., saccade) to the clock, is the perception of a slower-than-normal second-hand movement rate (the seconds hand of the clock may seemingly temporarily freeze in place after initially looking at it).[24][25][26][27]

The occurrence of chronostasis extends beyond the visual domain into the auditory and tactile domains.[28] In the auditory domain, chronostasis and duration overestimation occur when observing auditory stimuli. One common example is a frequent occurrence when making telephone calls. If, while listening to the phone's dial tone, research subjects move the phone from one ear to the other, the length of time between rings appears longer.[29] In the tactile domain, chronostasis has persisted in research subjects as they reach for and grasp objects. After grasping a new object, subjects overestimate the time in which their hand has been in contact with this object.[25] In other experiments, subjects turning a light on with a button were conditioned to experience the light before the button press.

Oddball effect

The perception of the duration of an event seems to be modulated by our recent experiences. Humans typically overestimate the perceived duration of the initial event in a stream of identical events[30] and unexpected “oddball” stimuli seem to be perceived as longer in duration, relative to expected or frequently presented “standard” stimuli.[31]

The oddball effect may serve an evolutionarily adapted “alerting” function and is consistent with reports of time slowing down in threatening situations. The effect seems to be strongest for images that are expanding in size on the retina, in other words, that are "looming" or approaching the viewer,[31][32][33] and the effect can be eradicated for oddballs that are contracting or perceived to be receding from the viewer.[32] The effect is also reduced[31] or reversed[33] with a static oddball presented amongst a stream of expanding stimuli.

Initial studies suggested that this oddball-induced “subjective time dilation” expanded the perceived duration of oddball stimuli by 30–50%[31] but subsequent research has reported more modest expansion of around 10%[33][34][35][36] or less.[37] The direction of the effect, whether the viewer perceives an increase or a decrease in duration, also seems to be dependent upon the stimulus used.[37]

Effects of emotional states

Awe

Research has suggested the feeling of awe has the ability to expand one's perceptions of time availability. Awe can be characterized as an experience of immense perceptual vastness that coincides with an increase in focus. Consequently, it is conceivable that one's temporal perception would slow down when experiencing awe.[38]

Fear

Possibly related to the oddball effect, research suggests that time seems to slow down for a person during dangerous events (such as a car accident, a robbery, or when a person perceives a potential predator or mate), or when a person skydives or bungee jumps, where they're capable of complex thoughts in what would normally be the blink of an eye (See Fight-or-flight response).[62] This reported slowing in temporal perception may have been evolutionarily advantageous because it may have enhanced one's ability to intelligibly make quick decisions in moments that were of critical importance to our survival.[39] However, even though observers commonly report that time seems to have moved in slow motion during these events, it is unclear whether this is a function of increased time resolution during the event, or instead an illusion created by the remembering of an emotionally salient event.[40]

A strong time dilation effect has been reported for perception of objects that were looming, but not of those retreating, from the viewer, suggesting that the expanding discs — which mimic an approaching object — elicit self-referential processes which act to signal the presence of a possible danger.[41] Anxious people, or those in great fear, experience greater "time dilation" in response to the same threat stimuli due to higher levels of epinephrine, which increases brain activity (an adrenaline rush).[42] In such circumstances, an illusion of time dilation could assist an efficacious escape.[43][44] When exposed to a threat, three-year-old children were observed to exhibit a similar tendency to overestimate elapsed time.[45][46]

Research suggests that the effect appears only at the point of retrospective assessment, rather than occurring simultaneously with events as they happened.[47] Perceptual abilities were tested during a frightening experience — a free fall — by measuring people's sensitivity to flickering stimuli. The results showed that the subjects' temporal resolution was not improved as the frightening event was occurring. Events appear to have taken longer only in retrospect, possibly because memories were being more densely packed during the frightening situation.[47]

People shown extracts from films known to induce fear often overestimated the elapsed time of a subsequently presented visual stimulus, whereas people shown emotionally neutral clips (weather forecasts and stock market updates) or those known to evoke feelings of sadness showed no difference. It is argued that fear prompts a state of arousal in the amygdala, which increases the rate of a hypothesized "internal clock". This could be the result of an evolved defensive mechanism triggered by a threatening situation.[48]

Empathy

The perception of another persons' emotions can also change our sense of time. The theory of embodied mind (or cognition), caused by mirror neurons, helps explain how the perception of other people's emotions has the ability to change one's own sense of time. Embodied cognition hinges on an internal process that mimics or simulates another's emotional state. For example, if person #1 spends time with person #2 who speaks and walks incredibly slowly, person #1's internal clock may slow down.

Depression

Depression may increase one's ability to perceive time accurately. One study assessed this concept by asking subjects to estimate the amount of time that passed during intervals ranging from 3 seconds to 65 seconds.[49] Results indicated that depressed subjects more accurately estimated the amount of time that had passed than non-depressed patients; non-depressed subjects overestimated the passing of time. This difference was hypothesized to be because depressed subjects focused less on external factors that may skew their judgment of time. The authors termed this hypothesized phenomenon "depressive realism."[49]

Changes with age

Psychologists have found that the subjective perception of the passing of time tends to speed up with increasing age in humans. This often causes people to increasingly underestimate a given interval of time as they age. This fact can likely be attributed to a variety of age-related changes in the aging brain, such as the lowering in dopaminergic levels with older age; however, the details are still being debated.[50][51][52] In an experimental study involving a group of subjects aged between 19 and 24 and a group between 60 and 80, the participants' abilities to estimate 3 minutes of time were compared. The study found that an average of 3 minutes and 3 seconds passed when participants in the younger group estimated that 3 minutes had passed, whereas the older group's estimate for when 3 minutes had passed came after an average of 3 minutes and 40 seconds.[79][53]

Very young children literally "live in time" before gaining an awareness of its passing. A child will first experience the passing of time when he or she can subjectively perceive and reflect on the unfolding of a collection of events. A child's awareness of time develops during childhood when the child's attention and short-term memory capacities form — this developmental process is thought to be dependent on the slow maturation of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus.[45][54]

One day to an 11-year-old would be approximately 1/4,000 of their life, while one day to a 55-year-old would be approximately 1/20,000 of their life. This helps to explain why a random, ordinary day may therefore appear longer for a young child than an adult.[55] The short term appears to go faster in proportion to the square root of the perceiver's age.[56] So a year would be experienced by a 55-year-old as passing approximately 2¼ times more quickly than a year experienced by an 11-year-old. If long-term time perception is based solely on the proportionality of a person's age, then the following four periods in life would appear to be quantitatively equal: ages 5–10 (1x), ages 10–20 (2x), ages 20–40 (4x), age 40–80 (8x).[55]

The common explanation is that most external and internal experiences are new for young children but repetitive for adults. Children have to be extremely engaged (i.e. dedicate many neural resources or significant brain power) in the present moment because they must constantly reconfigure their mental models of the world to assimilate it and manage behaviour properly. Adults however may rarely need to step outside mental habits and external routines. When an adult frequently experiences the same stimuli, they seem "invisible" because already sufficiently and effectively mapped by the brain. This phenomenon is known as neural adaptation. Thus, the brain will record fewer densely rich memories during these frequent periods of disengagement from the present moment.[57] Consequently, the subjective perception is often that time passes by at a faster rate with age.

Effects of drugs

Stimulants produce overestimates of time duration, whereas depressants and anaesthetics produce underestimates of time duration.

Psychoactive drugs can alter the judgment of time. These include traditional psychedelics such as LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline as well as the dissociative class of psychedelics such as PCP, ketamine and dextromethorphan. At higher doses time may appear to slow down, speed up or seem out of sequence. In a 2007 study, psilocybin was found to significantly impair the ability to reproduce interval durations longer than 2.5 seconds, significantly impair synchronizing motor actions (taps on a computer keyboard) with regularly occurring tones, and impair the ability to keep tempo when asked to tap on a key at a self-paced but consistent interval.[87]{{Unreliable medical source|sure=y|date=January 2014}} In 1955, British MP Christopher Mayhew took mescaline hydrochloride in an experiment under the guidance of his friend, Dr Humphry Osmond. On the BBC documentary The Beyond Within, he described that half a dozen times during the experiment, he had "a period of time that didn't end for [him]".

Stimulants can lead both humans and rats to overestimate time intervals,[88][89] while depressants can have the opposite effect.[90] The level of activity in the brain of neurotransmitters such as dopamine and norepinephrine may be the reason for this.[91] Dopamine has a particularly strong connection with one's perception of time. Drugs that activate dopamine receptors speed up one's perception of time, while dopamine antagonists cause one to feel that time is passing slowly.[45][58]

The effect of cannabis on time perception has been studied with inconclusive results.

Effects of body temperature

Time perception may speed up as body temperature rises, and slow down as body temperature lowers. This is especially true during stressful events.[94]

Reversal of temporal order judgment

Numerous experimental findings suggest that temporal order judgments of actions preceding effects can be reversed under special circumstances. Experiments have shown that sensory simultaneity judgments can be manipulated by repeated exposure to non-simultaneous stimuli. In an experiment conducted by David Eagleman, a temporal order judgment reversal was induced in subjects by exposing them to delayed motor consequences. In the experiment, subjects played various forms of video games. Unknown to the subjects, the experimenters introduced a fixed delay between the mouse movements and the subsequent sensory feedback. For example, a subject may not see a movement register on the screen until 150 milliseconds after the mouse had moved. Participants playing the game quickly adapted to the delay and felt as though there was less delay between their mouse movement and the sensory feedback. Shortly after the experimenters removed the delay, the subjects commonly felt as though the effect on the screen happened just before they commanded it. This work addresses how the perceived timing of effects is modulated by expectations, and the extent to which such predictions are quickly modifiable.[59] In an experiment conducted by Haggard and colleagues in 2002, participants pressed a button that triggered a flash of light at a distance after a slight delay of 100 milliseconds.[60] By repeatedly engaging in this act, participants had adapted to the delay (i.e., they experienced a gradual shortening in the perceived time interval between pressing the button and seeing the flash of light). The experimenters then showed the flash of light instantly after the button was pressed. In response, subjects often thought that the flash (the effect) had occurred before the button was pressed (the cause). Additionally, when the experimenters slightly reduced the delay, and shortened the spatial distance between the button and the flash of light, participants had often claimed again to have experienced the effect before the cause.

Several experiments also suggest that temporal order judgment of a pair of tactile stimuli delivered in rapid succession, one to each hand, is noticeably impaired (i.e., misreported) by crossing the hands over the midline. However, congenitally blind subjects showed no trace of temporal order judgment reversal after crossing the arms. These results suggest that tactile signals taken in by the congenitally blind are ordered in time without being referred to a visuospatial representation. Unlike the congenitally blind subjects, the temporal order judgments of the late-onset blind subjects were impaired when crossing the arms to a similar extent as non-blind subjects. These results suggest that the associations between tactile signals and visuospatial representation is maintained once it is accomplished during infancy. Some research studies have also found that the subjects showed reduced deficit in tactile temporal order judgments when the arms were crossed behind their back than when they were crossed in front.[61][62][63]

Flash-lag effect

{{Further|Flash lag illusion}}

In an experiment, participants were told to stare at an "x" symbol on a computer screen whereby a moving blue doughnut-like ring repeatedly circled the fixed "x" point.[64][65][66] Occasionally, the ring would display a white flash for a split second that physically overlapped the ring's interior. However, when asked what was perceived, participants responded that they saw the white flash lagging behind the center of the moving ring. In other words, despite the reality that the two retinal images were actually spatially aligned, the flashed object was usually observed to trail a continuously moving object in space — a phenomenon referred to as the flash-lag effect.

The first proposed explanation, called the 'motion extrapolation' hypothesis, is that the visual system extrapolates the position of moving objects but not flashing objects when accounting for neural delays (i.e., the lag time between the retinal image and the observer's perception of the flashing object). The second proposed explanation by David Eagleman and Sejnowski, called the 'latency difference' hypothesis, is that the visual system processes moving objects at a faster rate than flashed objects. In the attempt to disprove the first hypothesis, David Eagleman conducted an experiment in which the moving ring suddenly reverses direction to spin in the other way as the flashed object briefly appears. If the first hypothesis were correct, we would expect that, immediately following reversal, the moving object would be observed as lagging behind the flashed object. However, the experiment revealed the opposite — immediately following reversal, the flashed object was observed as lagging behind the moving object. This experimental result supports of the 'latency difference' hypothesis. A recent study tries to reconcile these different approaches by approaching perception as an inference mechanism aiming at describing what is happening at the present time.[67]

Effects of clinical disorders

Parkinson's disease,[104] schizophrenia,[105] and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)[106] have been linked to abnormalities in dopamine levels in the brain as well as to noticeable impairments in time perception. Neuropharmacological research indicates that the internal clock, used to time durations in the seconds-to-minutes range, is linked to dopamine function in the basal ganglia.[105] Studies in which children with ADHD are given time estimation tasks shows that time passes very slowly for them. Children with Tourette’s Syndrome, in contrast, who need to use the pre-frontal cortex to help them control their tics, are better at estimating intervals of time just over a second than other children.

In his book Awakenings, the neurologist Dr. Oliver Sacks discussed how patients with Parkinson's disease experience deficits in their awareness of time and tempo. For example, Mr E, when asked to clap his hands steadily and regularly, did so for the first few claps before clapping faster and irregularly, culminating in an apparent freezing of motion. When he finished, Mr E asked if his observers were glad he did it correctly, to which they replied "no". Mr E was offended by this because to him, his claps were regular and steady.[108]

Dopamine is also theorized to play a role in the attention deficits present with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Specifically, dopaminergic systems are involved in working memory and inhibitory processes, both of which are believed central to ADHD pathology.[106] Children with ADHD have also been found to be significantly impaired on time discrimination tasks (telling the difference between two stimuli of different temporal lengths) and respond earlier on time reproduction tasks (duplicating the duration of a presented stimulus) than controls.[110]

Along with other perceptual abnormalities, it has been noted by psychologists that schizophrenia patients have an altered sense of time. This was first described in psychology by Minkowski in 1927.[111] Many schizophrenic patients stop perceiving time as a flow of causally linked events. It has been suggested that there is usually a delay in time perception in schizophrenic patients compared to normal subjects.

These defects in time perception may play a part in the hallucinations and delusions experienced by schizophrenic patients according to some studies. Some researchers suggest that "abnormal timing judgment leads to a deficit in action attribution and action perception."[111]

Sleep

The perception of time is temporarily suspended during sleep, or more often during REM sleep. This can be attributed to the altered state of consciousness associated with sleep that prevents awareness of the surroundings, which would make it difficult to remain informed of the passing of time — new memories are rarely made during sleep. Therefore, upon waking up in the morning a person subjectively feels no time has passed but reasons that many hours have elapsed simply because it is now light outside. The passing of time must be inferred by observations of objects (e.g., the sun’s location, the moon, a clock's time) relative to the previous evening. So, time may feel as passing "faster" during sleep due to the lack of reference points. Another experience sometimes reported is a long dream seeming to go on for hours when it actually lasted only a few seconds or minutes.[68]

See also

  • Time
  • Arrow of time
  • Time dilation
  • Dyschronometria
  • Benjamin Libet
  • Temporal resolution

References

1. ^{{cite book | last1 = Evans | first1 = Vyvyan | name-list-format = vanc | title = Language and time: a cognitive linguistics approach | date = 2013 | publisher = Cambridge University Press | location = Cambridge | isbn = 978-1-107-04380-0 | url = https://books.google.com/books/about/Language_and_Time.html?id=2deaAAAAQBAJ }}
2. ^{{cite news |last=Livni |first=Ephrat |title=Physics explains why time passes faster as you age |url=https://qz.com/1516804/physics-explains-why-time-passes-faster-as-you-age/ |date=8 January 2019 |work=Quartz |accessdate=21 March 2019 }}
3. ^{{cite news |author=Duke University |title=It's spring already? Physics explains why time flies as we age - A slowdown in image processing speeds up our perception of time passing as we age |url=https://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2019-03/du-isa032019.php |date=21 March 2019 |work=EurekAlert! |accessdate=21 March 2019 }}
4. ^Karl Ernst von Baer, Welche Auffassung der lebenden Natur ist die richtige?, Berlin, 1862
5. ^{{cite book| vauthors = Friedman W | title = About time: inventing the fourth dimension| year = 1990 | publisher = MIT Press | location = Cambridge, Mass. | isbn = 978-0-262-06133-9 }}
6. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Friedman WJ | title = Memory for the time of past events | journal = Psychological Bulletin | year = 1993 | volume = 113 | issue = 1 | pages = 44–66 | doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.44 }}
7. ^{{cite journal | last = Falk | first = Dan | name-list-format = vanc | title = Do Humans Have a Biological Stopwatch? | journal = Smithsonian Magazine | url = http://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/do-humans-have-a-biological-stopwatch-164710819/?no-ist | date = Jan 2013 | accessdate = May 1, 2014 }}
8. ^Sichot HaRan 288, Likutey Tefillot II, 33.
9. ^Likutey Moharan II, 61
10. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Rao SM, Mayer AR, Harrington DL | title = The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 4 | issue = 3 | pages = 317–23 | date = March 2001 | pmid = 11224550 | doi = 10.1038/85191 | laydate = 27 February 2001 | laysource = UniSci: Daily University Science News | layurl = http://www.unisci.com/stories/20011/0227013.htm }}
11. ^{{cite journal | authors = Heron J, Aaen-Stockdale C, Hotchkiss J, Roach NW, McGraw PV, Whitaker D | title = Duration channels mediate human time perception | journal = Proceedings: Biological Sciences | volume = 279 | issue = 1729 | pages = 690–8 | date = February 2012 | pmid = 21831897 | pmc = 3248727 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.2011.1131 | df = }}
12. ^{{cite journal | authors=Heron J, Hotchkiss J, Aaen-Stockdale C, Roach N.W, & Whitaker, D. | year=2013 | title=A neural hierarchy for illusions of time: duration adaptation precedes multisensory integration. | journal=Journal of Vision | volume=13 | issue=14 | pages=4 | doi=10.1167/13.14.4| pmid=24306853 | pmc=3852255 }}
13. ^{{cite web | last1 = Eagleman | first1 = David M. | name-list-format = vanc | title = Brain Time | url = http://www.edge.org/conversation/brain-time | website = Edge | publisher = Edge Foundation | date = 23 June 2009 | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20131221170926/http://www.edge.org/conversation/brain-time | archivedate = 21 December 2013 | df = }}
14. ^{{cite book | last= Macey | first = Samuel L. | name-list-format =vanc | year = 1994 | title = Encyclopedia of Time | edition = 1st | publisher = Routledge Publishing | isbn = 978-0-8153-0615-3 | page=555 }}
15. ^{{cite book | last = Brockman | first = Max | name-list-format = vanc | year = 2009 | title = What's Next?: Dispatches on the Future of Science | publisher = Vintage Books | location = United States | isbn = 978-0-307-38931-2 | page = 162 }}
16. ^{{cite web | url = http://edge.org/conversation/brain-time | title = Brain Time | author = Eagleman DM | authorlink = David Eagleman | date = 2009-06-23 | website = | publisher = Edge Foundation | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20130805000331/http://www.edge.org/conversation/brain-time | archivedate = 2013-08-05 | df = }}
17. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Wearden JH, Todd NP, Jones LA | title = When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? | journal = Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 59 | issue = 10 | pages = 1709–24 | date = October 2006 | pmid = 16945856 | doi = 10.1080/17470210500314729 }}
18. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Goldstone S, Lhamon WT | title = Studies of auditory-visual differences in human time judgment. 1. Sounds are judged longer than lights | journal = Perceptual and Motor Skills | volume = 39 | issue = 1 | pages = 63–82 | date = August 1974 | pmid = 4415924 | doi = 10.2466/pms.1974.39.1.63 }}
19. ^{{cite book |vauthors=Penney TB |chapter=Modality differences in interval timing: Attention, clock speed, and memory |pages=209–233 |doi=10.1201/9780203009574.ch8 |title=Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing |volume=19 |editor-last=Meck | editor-first = Warren H. | name-list-format = vanc | date = 2003 | publisher = CRC Press |place=Boca Raton, FL |chapter-url=http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2003-00556-008 |series=Frontiers in Neuroscience | isbn = 978-0-8493-1109-3 }}
20. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Wearden JH, Edwards H, Fakhri M, Percival A | title = Why "sounds are judged longer than lights": application of a model of the internal clock in humans | journal = The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology | volume = 51 | issue = 2 | pages = 97–120 | date = May 1998 | pmid = 9621837 | doi = 10.1080/713932672 | url = http://www.keele.ac.uk/media/keeleuniversity/facnatsci/schpsych/weardenpublications/weardenetal1998.pdf | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20130421045901/http://www.keele.ac.uk/media/keeleuniversity/facnatsci/schpsych/weardenpublications/weardenetal1998.pdf | archivedate = 2013-04-21 | df = | doi-broken-date = 2019-02-20 }}
21. ^{{cite journal|last1=Goldreich|first1=Daniel|title=A Bayesian Perceptual Model Replicates the Cutaneous Rabbit and Other Tactile Spatiotemporal Illusions|journal=PLoS ONE|date=28 March 2007|volume=2|issue=3|pages=e333|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0000333|pmid=17389923|pmc=1828626}}
22. ^Cicchini G, Binda P and Morrone M (2009). A model for the distortions of space and time perception during saccadic movement. Front. Syst. Neurosci. Conference Abstract: Computational and systems neuroscience 2009. {{doi|10.3389/conf.neuro.06.2009.03.349}}
23. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Yarrow K, Whiteley L, Rothwell JC, Haggard P | title = Spatial consequences of bridging the saccadic gap | journal = Vision Research | volume = 46 | issue = 4 | pages = 545–55 | date = February 2006 | pmid = 16005489 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2005.04.019 | pmc=1343538}}
24. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Knöll J, Morrone MC, Bremmer F | title = Spatio-temporal topography of saccadic overestimation of time | journal = Vision Research | volume = 83 | pages = 56–65 | date = May 2013 | pmid = 23458677 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.013 }}
25. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Yarrow K, Rothwell JC | title = Manual chronostasis: tactile perception precedes physical contact | journal = Current Biology | volume = 13 | issue = 13 | pages = 1134–9 | date = July 2003 | pmid = 12842013 | doi = 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00413-5 }}
26. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Yarrow K, Johnson H, Haggard P, Rothwell JC | title = Consistent chronostasis effects across saccade categories imply a subcortical efferent trigger | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 16 | issue = 5 | pages = 839–47 | date = June 2004 | pmid = 15200711 | doi = 10.1162/089892904970780 | pmc=1266050}}
27. ^{{cite news | url=http://www.bbc.com/future/story/20120827-how-to-make-time-stand-still | title=The mystery of the stopped clock illusion | work=BBC - Future - Health - | date=2012-08-27 | accessdate=2012-12-09 | deadurl=no | archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20130120100128/http://www.bbc.com/future/story/20120827-how-to-make-time-stand-still | archivedate=2013-01-20 | df= }}
28. ^{{cite book | last = Nijhawan | first = Romi | name-list-format = vanc | title=Space and Time in Perception and Action|year=2010|publisher=Cambridge University Press|location=Cambridge, UK|isbn=978-0-521-86318-6}}
29. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Hodinott-Hill I, Thilo KV, Cowey A, Walsh V | title = Auditory chronostasis: hanging on the telephone | journal = Current Biology | volume = 12 | issue = 20 | pages = 1779–81 | date = October 2002 | pmid = 12401174 | doi = 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01219-8 }}
30. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Rose D, Summers J | title = Duration illusions in a train of visual stimuli | journal = Perception | volume = 24 | issue = 10 | pages = 1177–87 | year = 1995 | pmid = 8577576 | doi = 10.1068/p241177 }}
31. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Tse PU, Intriligator J, Rivest J, Cavanagh P | title = Attention and the subjective expansion of time | journal = Perception & Psychophysics | volume = 66 | issue = 7 | pages = 1171–89 | date = October 2004 | pmid = 15751474 | doi = 10.3758/BF03196844 }}
32. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = New JJ, Scholl BJ | title = Subjective time dilation: spatially local, object-based, or a global visual experience? | journal = Journal of Vision | volume = 9 | issue = 2 | pages = 4.1–11 | date = February 2009 | pmid = 19271914 | doi = 10.1167/9.2.4 }}
33. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = van Wassenhove V, Buonomano DV, Shimojo S, Shams L | title = Distortions of subjective time perception within and across senses | journal = PLoS One | volume = 3 | issue = 1 | pages = e1437 | date = January 2008 | pmid = 18197248 | pmc = 2174530 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0001437 }}
34. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Ulrich R, Nitschke J, Rammsayer T | title = Perceived duration of expected and unexpected stimuli | journal = Psychological Research | volume = 70 | issue = 2 | pages = 77–87 | date = March 2006 | pmid = 15609031 | doi = 10.1007/s00426-004-0195-4 }}
35. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Chen KM, Yeh SL | title = Asymmetric cross-modal effects in time perception | journal = Acta Psychologica | volume = 130 | issue = 3 | pages = 225–34 | date = March 2009 | pmid = 19195633 | doi = 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.12.008 }}
36. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Seifried T, Ulrich R | title = Does the asymmetry effect inflate the temporal expansion of odd stimuli? | journal = Psychological Research | volume = 74 | issue = 1 | pages = 90–8 | date = January 2010 | pmid = 19034503 | doi = 10.1007/s00426-008-0187-x }}
37. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Aaen-Stockdale C, Hotchkiss J, Heron J, Whitaker D | title = Perceived time is spatial frequency dependent | journal = Vision Research | volume = 51 | issue = 11 | pages = 1232–8 | date = June 2011 | pmid = 21477613 | pmc = 3121949 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.019 }}
38. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Rudd M, Vohs KD, Aaker J | title = Awe expands people's perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 23 | issue = 10 | pages = 1130–6 | date = October 2012 | pmid = 22886132 | doi = 10.1177/0956797612438731 | url = http://www.carlsonschool.umn.edu/assets/lib/assets/AssetLibrary/2012/Rudd_Vohs_Aaker_2012_psych%20science.pdf | citeseerx = 10.1.1.650.9416 }}
39. ^{{cite news|last1=Geoghagen|first1=Tom|title=Turn back the clock|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/uk_news/magazine/6926500.stm|publisher=BBC News Magazine|date=2007-08-02}}
40. ^[https://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-19477623 Why top sport stars might have 'more time' on the ball] by Jonathan Amos Science correspondent, BBC News
41. ^Eagleman, D. & Pariyadath, V. (2009). Is subjective duration a signature of coding efficiency? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 364: 1841-1851.
42. ^Yair Bar-Haim, Aya Kerem, Dominique Lamy & Dan Zakay (2010) When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety, Cognition and Emotion, 24:2, 255-263, DOI: 10.1080/02699930903387603
43. ^Tse, P.U., et al. (2004). Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Percept. Psychophys. 66: 1171-1189
44. ^[https://www.livescience.com/2117-time-slow-emergencies.html Why Time Seems to Slow Down in Emergencies] by By Charles Q. Choi, Live Science Contributor
45. ^{{cite web |url= https://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/jan/01/psychology-time-perception-awareness-research |title= A stopwatch on the brain's perception of time |last1= Gozlan |first1= Marc |date= 2 Jan 2013 |website= theguardian.com |publisher= Guardian News and Media Limited |accessdate= 4 January 2014 |name-list-format= vanc |deadurl= no |archiveurl= https://web.archive.org/web/20140104205501/http://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/jan/01/psychology-time-perception-awareness-research |archivedate= 4 January 2014 |df= }}
46. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Gil S, Droit-Volet S | title = Time perception, depression and sadness | journal = Behavioural Processes | volume = 80 | issue = 2 | pages = 169–76 | date = February 2009 | pmid = 19073237 | doi = 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.012 | url = http://cerca.labo.univ-poitiers.fr/IMG/pdf_BP09-GilColl.pdf | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20140104031908/http://cerca.labo.univ-poitiers.fr/IMG/pdf_BP09-GilColl.pdf | archivedate = 2014-01-04 | df = }}
47. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Stetson C, Fiesta MP, Eagleman DM | title = Does time really slow down during a frightening event? | journal = PLoS One | volume = 2 | issue = 12 | pages = e1295 | date = December 2007 | pmid = 18074019 | pmc = 2110887 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0001295 }}
48. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Droit-Volet S, Fayolle SL, Gil S | title = Emotion and time perception: effects of film-induced mood | journal = Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience | volume = 5 | issue = | pages = 33 | year = 2011 | pmid = 21886610 | pmc = 3152725 | doi = 10.3389/fnint.2011.00033 }}
49. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Kornbrot DE, Msetfi RM, Grimwood MJ | title = Time perception and depressive realism: judgment type, psychophysical functions and bias | journal = PLoS One | volume = 8 | issue = 8 | pages = e71585 | date = 21 August 2013 | pmid = 23990960 | pmc = 3749223 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0071585 | lay-url = https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/08/130822090326.htm | lay-date = 22 August 2013 | lay-source = Science Daily | df = }}
50. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Dreher JC, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn P, Berman KF | title = Age-related changes in midbrain dopaminergic regulation of the human reward system | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 105 | issue = 39 | pages = 15106–11 | date = September 2008 | pmid = 18794529 | pmc = 2567500 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0802127105 }}
51. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Bäckman L, Nyberg L, Lindenberger U, Li SC, Farde L | title = The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: current status and future prospects | journal = Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews | volume = 30 | issue = 6 | pages = 791–807 | year = 2006 | pmid = 16901542 | doi = 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.005 | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-FF03-0 }}
52. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Meck WH | title = Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception | journal = Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research | volume = 3 | issue = 3–4 | pages = 227–42 | date = June 1996 | pmid = 8806025 | doi = 10.1016/0926-6410(96)00009-2 | url = http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homePage/Class/Psy355/Gilden/meck.pdf | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20131029135616/http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/class/Psy355/Gilden/meck.pdf | archivedate = 2013-10-29 | df = }}
53. ^{{cite web | url = https://www.nytimes.com/1998/03/24/science/running-late-researchers-blame-aging-brain.html?pagewanted=all&src=pm | title = Running Late? Researchers Blame Aging Brain | author = Blakesell S | date = 1998-03-24 | website = | publisher = New York Times | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20170813013945/http://www.nytimes.com/1998/03/24/science/running-late-researchers-blame-aging-brain.html?pagewanted=all&src=pm | archivedate = 2017-08-13 | df = }}
54. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Kolb B, Mychasiuk R, Muhammad A, Li Y, Frost DO, Gibb R | title = Experience and the developing prefrontal cortex | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 109 Suppl 2 | issue = | pages = 17186–93 | date = October 2012 | pmid = 23045653 | pmc = 3477383 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.1121251109 }}
55. ^{{cite web | url = https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg16422180.900-look-how-time-flies | title = Look how time flies . . . | author = Adler R | date = 1999-12-25 | publisher = New Scientist | accessdate = 2009-10-22 | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20110614233936/http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg16422180.900-look-how-time-flies | archivedate = 2011-06-14 | df = }}
56. ^{{cite news | last1 = Jo DiLonardo | first1 = Mary | name-list-format = vanc | title = Time Does Fly As We Grow Older | url = http://articles.chicagotribune.com/1994-02-06/features/9402060210_1_psychology-professor-adult-school-year | work = Chicago Tribune | date = 1994-02-06 | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20160425120403/http://articles.chicagotribune.com/1994-02-06/features/9402060210_1_psychology-professor-adult-school-year | archivedate = 2016-04-25 | df = }}
57. ^{{cite web | url = http://blog.bufferapp.com/the-science-of-time-perception-how-to-make-your-days-longer | title = The science of time perception: stop it slipping away by doing new things | author = Cooper BB | date = 2013-07-02 | website = | publisher = The Buffer Blog | deadurl = no | archiveurl = http://archive.wikiwix.com/cache/20130816194820/http://blog.bufferapp.com/the-science-of-time-perception-how-to-make-your-days-longer | archivedate = 2013-08-16 | df = }}
58. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Rammsayer T | title = Is there a common dopaminergic basis of time perception and reaction time? | journal = Neuropsychobiology | volume = 21 | issue = 1 | pages = 37–42 | year = 1989 | pmid = 2573003 | doi = 10.1159/000118549 }}
59. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Stetson C, Cui X, Montague PR, Eagleman DM | title = Motor-sensory recalibration leads to an illusory reversal of action and sensation | journal = Neuron | volume = 51 | issue = 5 | pages = 651–9 | date = September 2006 | pmid = 16950162 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.006 | url = http://www.eaglemanlab.net/papers/StetsonetalNeuron2006.pdf | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20130928021519/http://www.eaglemanlab.net/papers/StetsonetalNeuron2006.pdf | archivedate = 2013-09-28 | df = }}
60. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Eagleman DM | title = Human time perception and its illusions | journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology | volume = 18 | issue = 2 | pages = 131–6 | date = April 2008 | pmid = 18639634 | pmc = 2866156 | doi = 10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002 }}
61. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S | title = Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 4 | issue = 7 | pages = 759–65 | date = July 2001 | pmid = 11426234 | doi = 10.1038/89559 | url = http://wexler.free.fr/library/files/yamamoto%20(2001)%20reversal%20of%20subjective%20temporal%20order%20due%20to%20arm%20crossing.pdf | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20150402142713/http://wexler.free.fr/library/files/yamamoto%20(2001)%20reversal%20of%20subjective%20temporal%20order%20due%20to%20arm%20crossing.pdf | archivedate = 2015-04-02 | df = }}
62. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Sambo CF, Torta DM, Gallace A, Liang M, Moseley GL, Iannetti GD | title = The temporal order judgement of tactile and nociceptive stimuli is impaired by crossing the hands over the body midline | journal = Pain | volume = 154 | issue = 2 | pages = 242–7 | date = February 2013 | pmid = 23200703 | doi = 10.1016/j.pain.2012.10.010 | url = http://www.bodyinmind.org/wp-content/uploads/1-s2.0-S0304395912005611-main.pdf | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20130928081457/http://www.bodyinmind.org/wp-content/uploads/1-s2.0-S0304395912005611-main.pdf | archivedate = 2013-09-28 | df = }}
63. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Takahashi T, Kansaku K, Wada M, Shibuya S, Kitazawa S | title = Neural correlates of tactile temporal-order judgment in humans: an fMRI study | journal = Cerebral Cortex | volume = 23 | issue = 8 | pages = 1952–64 | date = August 2013 | pmid = 22761307 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/bhs179 }}
64. ^{{cite web | last1 = Kotler | first1 = Steven | name-list-format = vanc | title = When Life Flashes Before Your Eyes: A 15-Story Drop to Study the Brain's Internal Timewarp | url = http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2010-03/how-time-flies | website = Popular Science | publisher = Bonnier Corporation | date = 12 April 2010 | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20141011234410/http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2010-03/how-time-flies | archivedate = 11 October 2014 | df = }}
65. ^{{cite web | last1 = Eagleman | first1 = DM | last2 = Sejnowski | first2 = TJ | name-list-format = vanc | title = Flash-Lag Effect | url = http://www.eaglemanlab.net/flashlag | website = Eagleman Laboratory for Perception and Action | date = 2007 | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20140801091656/http://eaglemanlab.net/flashlag/ | archivedate = 2014-08-01 | df = }}
66. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Patel SS, Ogmen H, Bedell HE, Sampath V | title = Flash-lag effect: differential latency, not postdiction | journal = Science | volume = 290 | issue = 5494 | pages = 1051 | date = November 2000 | pmid = 11184992 | doi = | url = http://eaglemanlab.net/papers/EagleSejScience3.pdf | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20140808055953/http://eaglemanlab.net/papers/EagleSejScience3.pdf | dead-url = yes | archive-date = 2014-08-08 }}
67. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Khoei MA, Masson GS, Perrinet LU | title = The flash-lag effect as a motion-based predictive shift | journal = PLoS Computational Biology | volume = 13 | issue = 1 | pages = e1005068 | date = January 2017 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005068 | pmid = 28125585 | pmc = 5268412 | df = }}
68. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.sciencefocus.com/article/human-body-mind/why-does-time-go-so-fast-when-you%E2%80%99re-asleep|title=Why does time go so fast when you're asleep?|date=7 February 2016|work=BBC Science Focus|accessdate=4 July 2018}}
69. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Arzy S, Molnar-Szakacs I, Blanke O | title = Self in time: imagined self-location influences neural activity related to mental time travel | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 28 | issue = 25 | pages = 6502–7 | date = June 2008 | pmid = 18562621 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5712-07.2008 }}
70. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Cheng RK, MacDonald CJ, Meck WH | title = Differential effects of cocaine and ketamine on time estimation: implications for neurobiological models of interval timing | journal = Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior | volume = 85 | issue = 1 | pages = 114–22 | date = September 2006 | pmid = 16920182 | doi = 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.07.019 }}
71. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Davalos DB, Kisley MA, Ross RG | title = Deficits in auditory and visual temporal perception in schizophrenia | journal = Cognitive Neuropsychiatry | volume = 7 | issue = 4 | pages = 273–82 | date = November 2002 | pmid = 16571542 | doi = 10.1080/13546800143000230 }}
72. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Franck N, Posada A, Pichon S, Haggard P | title = Altered subjective time of events in schizophrenia | journal = The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease | volume = 193 | issue = 5 | pages = 350–3 | date = May 2005 | pmid = 15870620 | doi = 10.1097/01.nmd.0000161699.76032.09 }}
73. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Jaldow EJ, Oakley DA, Davey GC | title = Performance of Decorticated Rats on Fixed Interval and Fixed Time Schedules | journal = The European Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 1 | issue = 5 | pages = 461–470 | date = September 1989 | pmid = 12106131 | doi = 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1989.tb00352.x }}
74. ^{{cite book | vauthors = James W | date = 1893 | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=JLcAAAAAMAAJ | title = The principles of psychology | location = New York | publisher = H. Holt and Company. | page = 609 }}
75. ^{{cite journal | title=It Seems Like Only Yesterday: The Nature and Consequences of Telescoping Errors in Marketing Research | publisher=Journal of Consumer Psychology | url=https://www.questia.com/googleScholar.qst;jsessionid=KgPbgnYGLQyyHQtdPQbVMGHm5wp3tr94mQLkBF4hTvYJ09dlJx5N!-1941634607!-2081211395?docId=77055094%7C | deadurl=no | archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20120701102333/http://www.questia.com/googleScholar.qst | archivedate=2012-07-01 | df= }}
76. ^{{cite web |url = http://content.yudu.com/Library/A2ac0l/JustCromerJuly132013/resources/index.htm?referrerUrl=http%3A%2F%2Ffree.yudu.com%2Fitem%2Fdetails%2F1044663%2FJust-Cromer-July-13--2013 |title = David dives in |date = 13 Jul 2013 |accessdate = 13 July 2013 |publisher = justRegional publishing |deadurl = no |archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20160826013530/http://content.yudu.com/Library/A2ac0l/JustCromerJuly132013/resources/index.htm?referrerUrl=http%3A%2F%2Ffree.yudu.com%2Fitem%2Fdetails%2F1044663%2FJust-Cromer-July-13--2013 |archivedate = 26 August 2016 |df = }}
77. ^Anonymous (E. Robert Kelly), The Alternative: A Study in Psychology. London: Macmillan and Co.,1882.
78. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Levy F, Swanson JM | title = Timing, space and ADHD: the dopamine theory revisited | journal = The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 35 | issue = 4 | pages = 504–11 | date = August 2001 | pmid = 11531733 | doi = 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00923.x }}
79. ^{{cite book | vauthors = Mackintosh NJ | title = Animal learning and cognition | publisher = Academic Press | location = Boston | year = 1994 | pages = | isbn = 978-0-12-161953-4 }}
80. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Wearden JH, Penton-Voak IS | title = Feeling the heat: body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited | journal = The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology | volume = 48 | issue = 2 | pages = 129–41 | date = May 1995 | pmid = 7597195 | doi = }}
81. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Holmes B | title = Why time flies in old age | journal = New Scientist | volume = | issue = 2057 | pages = | date = November 1996 | url = https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg15220571.700-why-time-flies-in-old-age.html | deadurl = no | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20150426130602/http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg15220571.700-why-time-flies-in-old-age.html | archivedate = 2015-04-26 | df = }}
82. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Pastor MA, Artieda J, Jahanshahi M, Obeso JA | title = Time estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson's disease | journal = Brain | volume = 115 | issue = 1 | pages = 211–25 | date = February 1992 | pmid = 1559155 | doi = 10.1093/brain/115.1.211 }}
83. ^"The Specious Present: A Neurophenomenology of Time Consciousness." In Petitot, Varela, Pacoud & Roy (eds.), Naturalizing Phenomenology. Stanford University Press.
84. ^{{cite web|url=http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/time-experience| author = Le Poidevin R | title = The Experience and Perception of Time|accessdate=2009-10-22}}
85. ^{{cite book | vauthors = Sacks OW | title = Awakenings | publisher = Vintage Books | location = New York | year = 1999 | pages = | isbn = 978-0-375-70405-5 }}
86. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Smith A, Taylor E, Rogers JW, Newman S, Rubia K | title = Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD | journal = Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines | volume = 43 | issue = 4 | pages = 529–42 | date = May 2002 | pmid = 12030598 | doi = 10.1111/1469-7610.00043 }}
87. ^{{vcite2 journal | vauthors = Terao M, Watanabe J, Yagi A, Nishida S | title = Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals | journal = Nat. Neurosci. | volume = 11 | issue = 5 | pages = 541–2 |date=May 2008 | pmid = 18408716 | doi = 10.1038/nn.2111 }}
88. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Tinklenberg JR, Roth WT, Kopell BS | title = Marijuana and ethanol: differential effects on time perception, heart rate, and subjective response | journal = Psychopharmacology | volume = 49 | issue = 3 | pages = 275–9 | date = September 1976 | pmid = 826945 | doi = 10.1007/BF00426830 }}
89. ^Wada Y, Masuda T, Noguchi K, 2005, "Temporal illusion called 'kappa effect' in event perception" Perception 34 ECVP Abstract Supplement
90. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Wittmann M, Carter O, Hasler F, Cahn BR, Grimberg U, Spring P, Hell D, Flohr H, Vollenweider FX | title = Effects of psilocybin on time perception and temporal control of behaviour in humans | journal = Journal of Psychopharmacology | volume = 21 | issue = 1 | pages = 50–64 | date = January 2007 | pmid = 16714323 | doi = 10.1177/0269881106065859 }}
91. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Wittmann M, Leland DS, Churan J, Paulus MP | title = Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects | journal = Drug and Alcohol Dependence | volume = 90 | issue = 2–3 | pages = 183–92 | date = October 2007 | pmid = 17434690 | pmc = 1997301 | doi = 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.005 }}
92. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Yarrow K, Haggard P, Heal R, Brown P, Rothwell JC | title = Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity | journal = Nature | volume = 414 | issue = 6861 | pages = 302–5 | date = November 2001 | pmid = 11713528 | doi = 10.1038/35104551 | url = http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/333/2/Illusory_perceptions_of_space_and_time_preserve_cross-saccadic_perceptual_continuity.pdf }}
93. ^{{cite journal | vauthors = Rao SM, Mayer AR, Harrington DL | title = The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 4 | issue = 3 | pages = 317–23 | date = March 2001 | pmid = 11224550 | doi = 10.1038/85191 | laysource = Nature Neuroscience | layurl = http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v4/n3/abs/nn0301_317.htm }}
[69][70][71][72][73][74][75][76][77][78][79][80][81][82][83][84][85][86][88][89][90][91][92][93]
}}

Further reading

{{refbegin|33em}}
  • {{cite web | last = Le Poidevin | first = Robin | name-list-format = vanc | url = http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2004/entries/time-experience/ | title = The Experience and Perception of Time | work = The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy | date = Winter 2004 | editor-first = Edward N. | editor-last = Zalta }}
  • {{cite book | vauthors = Hodder A | date = 1901 | title = The adversaries of the sceptic; or, The specious present, a new inquiry into human knowledge | chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=uZ5RAAAAMAAJ&pg=PA36&source=gbs_toc_r&cad=0_0 | chapter = Chapter II, The Specious Present | location = London: | publisher = S. Sonnenschein &. | pages = 36–56 }}
  • {{cite journal | vauthors = Underwood G, Swain RA | title = Selectivity of attention and the perception of duration | journal = Perception | volume = 2 | issue = 1 | pages = 101–5 | year = 1973 | pmid = 4777562 | doi = 10.1068/p020101 }}
  • {{cite journal | vauthors = Brown SW, Stubbs DA | title = Attention and interference in prospective and retrospective timing | journal = Perception | volume = 21 | issue = 4 | pages = 545–57 | year = 1992 | pmid = 1437469 | doi = 10.1068/p210545 }}
  • {{cite journal | vauthors = Eagleman DM, Tse PU, Buonomano D, Janssen P, Nobre AC, Holcombe AO | title = Time and the brain: how subjective time relates to neural time | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 25 | issue = 45 | pages = 10369–71 | date = November 2005 | pmid = 16280574 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3487-05.2005 }}
  • {{cite journal | vauthors = Slanger TG | title = Evidence for a Short-Period Internal Clock in Humans | journal = Journal of Scientific Exploration | year = 1988 | volume = 2 | issue = 2 | pages = 203–216 | url = http://www.scientificexploration.org/journal/jse_02_2_slanger.pdf | accessdate = 2011-10-02 | deadurl = yes | archiveurl = https://web.archive.org/web/20110809232323/http://www.scientificexploration.org/journal/jse_02_2_slanger.pdf | archivedate = 2011-08-09 | df = }}
  • {{cite book | vauthors = Le Poidevin R | title = The images of time: an essay on temporal representation | publisher = Oxford University Press | location = Oxford [Oxfordshire] | year = 2007 | pages = | isbn = 978-0-19-926589-3 | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=l0qrewXYnOYC&lpg=PA115&dq | authorlink = Robin LePoidevin }}
{{refend}}

External links

{{Wikibooks}}
  • The Logarithmic Time Perception Hypothesis
  • NeuroQuantology Special Issue: Time, Timing, and the Brain
  • Time perception research at the University of Manchester
  • Time Sense: Polychronicity and Monochronicity
  • "A Cognitive Model of Retrospective Duration Estimations", Hee-Kyung Ahn, et al., March 7, 2006.
  • "Time, Force, Motion, and the Semantics of Natural Languages", Wolfgang Wildgen, Antwerp Papers in Linguistics, 2003/2004.
  • [https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=RjlpamhrId8 Can Time Slow Down?]
  • [https://web.archive.org/web/20080122145356/http://www.pjonline.com/Editorial/20051119/comment/onlooker.html "Interactions emerge between biological clocks", The Pharmaceutical Journal, Vol 275 No 7376 p644, 19 November 2005] Registration required.
  • Picture Space Time helps to add Time Perception to Photographs using sound
{{Time Topics}}{{DEFAULTSORT:Time Perception}}

4 : Perception|Time|Unsolved problems in neuroscience|Time management

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/10 15:43:42