词条 | Matthew effect |
释义 |
The Matthew effect, Matthew principle, or Matthew effect of accumulated advantage can be observed in many aspects of life and fields of activity. It is sometimes summarized by the adage "the rich get richer and the poor get poorer."[1][2] The concept is applicable to matters of fame or status, but may also be applied literally to cumulative advantage of economic capital. The term was coined by sociologist Robert K. Merton in 1968[3] and takes its name from the Parable of the talents or minas in the biblical Gospel of Matthew. Merton credited his collaborator and wife, sociologist Harriet Zuckerman, as co-author of the concept of the Matthew effect.[3] EtymologyThe concept is named according to two of the Parables of Jesus in the synoptic Gospels (Table 2, of the Eusebian Canons). The concept concludes both synoptic versions of the parable of the talents: {{quote|For to every one who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Matthew 25:29, RSV.|source=}}{{quote|I tell you, that to every one who has will more be given; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Luke 19:26, RSV.|source=}}The concept concludes two of the three synoptic versions of the parable of the lamp under a bushel (absent in the version of Matthew): {{quote|For to him who has will more be given; and from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away|Mark 4:25, RSV.|source=}}{{quote|Take heed then how you hear; for to him who has will more be given, and from him who has not, even what he thinks that he has will be taken away.|Luke 8:18, RSV.|source=}}The concept is presented again in Matthew outside of a parable during Christ's explanation to his disciples of the purpose of parables: {{quote|And he answered them, "To you it has been given to know the secrets of the kingdom of heaven, but to them it has not been given. For to him who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away."|Matthew 13:11–12, RSV.|source=}}==Sociology of science== In the sociology of science, "Matthew effect" was a term coined by Robert K. Merton to describe how, among other things, eminent scientists will often get more credit than a comparatively unknown researcher, even if their work is similar; it also means that credit will usually be given to researchers who are already famous.[4][5] For example, a prize will almost always be awarded to the most senior researcher involved in a project, even if all the work was done by a graduate student. This was later formulated by Stephen Stigler as Stigler's law of eponymy – "No scientific discovery is named after its original discoverer" – with Stigler explicitly naming Merton as the true discoverer, making his "law" an example of itself. Merton furthermore argued that in the scientific community the Matthew effect reaches beyond simple reputation to influence the wider communication system, playing a part in social selection processes and resulting in a concentration of resources and talent. He gave as an example the disproportionate visibility given to articles from acknowledged authors, at the expense of equally valid or superior articles written by unknown authors. He also noted that the concentration of attention on eminent individuals can lead to an increase in their self-assurance, pushing them to perform research in important but risky problem areas.[6] Examples{{Original research|section|date=January 2012}}As credit is valued in science, specific claims of the Matthew effect are contentious. Many examples below exemplify more famous scientists getting credit for discoveries due to their fame, even as other less notable scientists had preempted their work.
Ray Solomonoff [...] introduced [what is now known as] "Kolmogorov complexity" in a long journal paper in 1964. [...] This makes Solomonoff the first inventor and raises the question whether we should talk about Solomonoff complexity. [...]
In science, dramatic differences in the productivity may be explained by three phenomena: sacred spark, cumulative advantage, and search costs minimization by journal editors. The sacred spark paradigm suggests that scientists differ in their initial abilities, talent, skills, persistence, work habits, etc. that provide particular individuals with an early advantage. These factors have a multiplicative effect which helps these scholars succeed later. The cumulative advantage model argues that an initial success helps a researcher gain access to resources (e.g., teaching release, best graduate students, funding, facilities, etc.), which in turn results in further success. Search costs minimization by journal editors takes place when editors try to save time and effort by consciously or subconsciously selecting articles from well-known scholars. Whereas the exact mechanism underlying these phenomena is yet unknown, it is documented that a minority of all academics produce the most research output and attract the most citations.[13] EducationIn education, the term "Matthew effect" has been adopted by psychologist Keith Stanovich to describe a phenomenon observed in research on how new readers acquire the skills to read: early success in acquiring reading skills usually leads to later successes in reading as the learner grows, while failing to learn to read before the third or fourth year of schooling may be indicative of lifelong problems in learning new skills. This is because children who fall behind in reading would read less, increasing the gap between them and their peers. Later, when students need to "read to learn" (where before they were learning to read), their reading difficulty creates difficulty in most other subjects. In this way they fall further and further behind in school, dropping out at a much higher rate than their peers. In the words of Stanovich: {{quote|Slow reading acquisition has cognitive, behavioral, and motivational consequences that slow the development of other cognitive skills and inhibit performance on many academic tasks. In short, as reading develops, other cognitive processes linked to it track the level of reading skill. Knowledge bases that are in reciprocal relationships with reading are also inhibited from further development. The longer this developmental sequence is allowed to continue, the more generalized the deficits will become, seeping into more and more areas of cognition and behavior. Or to put it more simply – and sadly – in the words of a tearful nine-year-old, already falling frustratingly behind his peers in reading progress, "Reading affects everything you do."[14]}}Network scienceIn network science, the Matthew effect is used to describe the preferential attachment of earlier nodes in a network, which explains that these nodes tend to attract more links early on.[15] "Because of preferential attachment, a node that acquires more connections than another one will increase its connectivity at a higher rate, and thus an initial difference in the connectivity between two nodes will increase further as the network grows, while the degree of individual nodes will grow proportional with the square root of time."[16] The Matthew Effect therefore explains the growth of some nodes in vast networks such as the Internet.[17] Markets with social influenceProduct recommendations and information about past purchases have been shown{{by whom?|date=September 2018}} to influence consumers' choices significantly - whether for music, movie, book, technological, and other types of products. Social influence often induces a rich-get-richer phenomenon where popular products tend to become even more popular.[18] See also
References1. ^{{Cite book| edition = 1| publisher = Little, Brown and Company| isbn = 0-316-01792-2| last = Gladwell| first = Malcolm| title = Outliers: The Story of Success| date = 2008-11-18}} 2. ^{{cite web |accessdate=2009-01-12 |url=https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB122671469296530435 |title=The Elements of Success |work=The Wall Street Journal |date=2008-11-15 |author=Shaywitz, David A. }} 3. ^http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthewii.pdf 4. ^1 {{cite journal |author=Merton, Robert K. |year=1968 |url=http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthew1.pdf |title=The Matthew Effect in Science |journal=Science |volume=159 |issue=3810 |pages=56–63 |doi=10.1126/science.159.3810.56 |pmid=17737466|bibcode=1968Sci...159...56M }} 5. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Merton | first1 = Robert K | year = 1988 | title = The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative advantage and the symbolism of intellectual property | url = http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthewii.pdf| journal = ISIS | volume = 79 | issue = | pages = 606–623 | doi=10.1086/354848}} 6. ^Abstract of Merton's 1968 paper "The Matthew Effect in Science". 7. ^Salganik, Matthew J., Peter S. Dodds, and Duncan J. Watts. (2006). Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market (PDF). Science 311 , 854–856. 8. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Sorenson | first1 = Alan T | year = 2007 | title = Bestseller Lists and Product Variety | url = http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~sorensen/papers/sorensen_JIE_2007.pdf | format = PDF | journal = Journal of Industrial Economics | volume = 55 | issue = | pages = 715–738 }} 9. ^{{cite journal | last1 = van de Rijt | first1 = A. | last2 = Kang | first2 = S. | last3 = Restivo | first3 = M. | last4 = Patil | first4 = A. | year = 2014 | title = Field Experiments of Success-Breeds-Success Dynamics | url = http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/111/19/6934.full.pdf | format = PDF | journal = PNAS | volume = 111 | issue = 19 | pages = 6934–6939 }} 10. ^{{Cite book| edition = 2nd| publisher = Springer| isbn = 0-387-94868-6| last = Li| first = Ming|author2=Paul Vitanyi| title = An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications| date = 1997-02-27}} 11. ^{{Cite journal |last=Petersen |first=Alexander M. |author2=Jung, Woo-Sung |author3=Yang, Jae-Suk |author4=Stanley, H. Eugene |year= 2011 |title=Quantitative and Empirical demonstration of the Matthew Effect in a study of Career Longevity |journal=PNAS |volume=108 |issue=1 |pages=18–23 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1016733108 |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/108/1/18.short |quote= |pmid= 21173276|pmc= 3017158|postscript= |arxiv=0806.1224 |bibcode=2011PNAS..108...18P }} 12. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Bol | first1 = T. | last2 = de Vaan | first2 = M. | last3 = van de Rijt | first3 = A. | year = 2018 | title = The Matthew Effect in Science Funding | url = http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/115/19/4887.full.pdf | format = PDF | journal = PNAS | volume = 115 | issue = 19 | pages = 4887–4890 }} 13. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Serenko | first1 = A. | last2 = Cox | first2 = R. | last3 = Bontis | first3 = N. | last4 = Booker | first4 = L. | year = 2011 | title = The Superstar Phenomenon in the Knowledge Management and Intellectual Capital Academic Discipline | url = http://www.aserenko.com/papers/JOI_Serenko_Superstar_Phenomenon_Published.pdf | format = PDF | journal = Journal of Informetrics | volume = 5 | issue = | pages = 333–345 }} 14. ^Adams, Marilyn J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 59–60. 15. ^{{Cite journal|last=Barabási|first=A-L|year=1999|title=Emergence of scaling in random networks|journal=Science|volume=286|pages=509–512|doi=10.1126/science.286.5439.509|pmc=|pmid=10521342|quote=|postscript=|authorlink=|author2=Albert, R|accessdate=|arxiv=cond-mat/9910332|bibcode=1999Sci...286..509B}} 16. ^{{Cite journal|last=Perc|first=Matjaž|year=2014|title=The Matthew effect in empirical data|url=http://rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/11/98/20140378|journal=Interface|volume=12|issue=104|pages=20140378|doi=10.1098/rsif.2014.0378|pmc=|pmid=|quote=|postscript=|authorlink=|accessdate=|arxiv=1408.5124}} 17. ^{{Cite book|title=Networks, Complexity And Internet Regulation – Scale-Free Law|last=Guadamuz|first=Andres|date=2011|publisher=Edward Elgar|isbn=9781848443105}} 18. ^{{cite journal | url= http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0180040 | title= Transient dynamics in trial-offer markets with social influence: Trade-offs between appeal and quality | author1= Altszyler, E |author2= Berbeglia, F. | author3= Berbeglia, G. |author4= Van Hentenryck, P. | journal= PLoS ONE | year= 2017 |volume= 12 |issue= 7 | doi= 10.1371/journal.pone.0180040 | page= e0180040 |bibcode= 2017PLoSO..1280040A}} Further reading{{Refbegin|30em}}
5 : Adages|Social phenomena|Sociological terminology|Words and phrases introduced in 1968|Gospel of Matthew |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。