请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Matthew effect
释义

  1. Etymology

     Examples 

  2. Education

  3. Network science

  4. Markets with social influence

  5. See also

  6. References

  7. Further reading

{{short description| Effect originally observed by Robert K. Merton }}

The Matthew effect, Matthew principle, or Matthew effect of accumulated advantage can be observed in many aspects of life and fields of activity. It is sometimes summarized by the adage "the rich get richer and the poor get poorer."[1][2] The concept is applicable to matters of fame or status, but may also be applied literally to cumulative advantage of economic capital.

The term was coined by sociologist Robert K. Merton in 1968[3] and takes its name from the Parable of the talents or minas in the biblical Gospel of Matthew. Merton credited his collaborator and wife, sociologist Harriet Zuckerman, as co-author of the concept of the Matthew effect.[3]

Etymology

The concept is named according to two of the Parables of Jesus in the synoptic Gospels (Table 2, of the Eusebian Canons).

The concept concludes both synoptic versions of the parable of the talents:

{{quote|For to every one who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Matthew 25:29, RSV.|source=}}{{quote|I tell you, that to every one who has will more be given; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Luke 19:26, RSV.|source=}}

The concept concludes two of the three synoptic versions of the parable of the lamp under a bushel (absent in the version of Matthew):

{{quote|For to him who has will more be given; and from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away|Mark 4:25, RSV.|source=}}{{quote|Take heed then how you hear; for to him who has will more be given, and from him who has not, even what he thinks that he has will be taken away.|Luke 8:18, RSV.|source=}}

The concept is presented again in Matthew outside of a parable during Christ's explanation to his disciples of the purpose of parables:

{{quote|And he answered them, "To you it has been given to know the secrets of the kingdom of heaven, but to them it has not been given. For to him who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away."|Matthew 13:11–12, RSV.|source=}}

==Sociology of science==

In the sociology of science, "Matthew effect" was a term coined by Robert K. Merton to describe how, among other things, eminent scientists will often get more credit than a comparatively unknown researcher, even if their work is similar; it also means that credit will usually be given to researchers who are already famous.[4][5] For example, a prize will almost always be awarded to the most senior researcher involved in a project, even if all the work was done by a graduate student. This was later formulated by Stephen Stigler as Stigler's law of eponymy – "No scientific discovery is named after its original discoverer"  – with Stigler explicitly naming Merton as the true discoverer, making his "law" an example of itself.

Merton furthermore argued that in the scientific community the Matthew effect reaches beyond simple reputation to influence the wider communication system, playing a part in social selection processes and resulting in a concentration of resources and talent. He gave as an example the disproportionate visibility given to articles from acknowledged authors, at the expense of equally valid or superior articles written by unknown authors. He also noted that the concentration of attention on eminent individuals can lead to an increase in their self-assurance, pushing them to perform research in important but risky problem areas.[6]

Examples

{{Original research|section|date=January 2012}}

As credit is valued in science, specific claims of the Matthew effect are contentious. Many examples below exemplify more famous scientists getting credit for discoveries due to their fame, even as other less notable scientists had preempted their work.

  • Experiments manipulating download counts or bestseller lists for books and music have shown consumer activity follows the apparent popularity.[7][8][9]
  • In algorithmic information theory, the notion of Kolmogorov complexity is named after the famous mathematician Andrey Kolmogorov even though it was independently discovered and published by Ray Solomonoff a year before Kolmogorov. Li and Vitanyi, in "An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications" (p. 84), write:[10]

Ray Solomonoff [...] introduced [what is now known as] "Kolmogorov complexity" in a long journal paper in 1964. [...] This makes Solomonoff the first inventor and raises the question whether we should talk about Solomonoff complexity. [...]

  • There are many uncontroversial examples of the Matthew effect in mathematics, where a concept is due to one mathematician (and well-documented as such), but is attributed to a later (possibly much later), more famous mathematician who worked on it. For instance, the Poincaré disk model and Poincaré half-plane model of hyperbolic space are both named for Henri Poincaré, but were introduced by Eugenio Beltrami in 1868 (when Poincaré was 14 and had not as yet contributed to hyperbolic geometry).
  • A model for career progress quantitatively incorporates the Matthew Effect in order to predict the distribution of individual career length in competitive professions. The model predictions are validated by analyzing the empirical distributions of career length for careers in science and professional sports (e.g. Major League Baseball).[11] As a result, the disparity between the large number of short careers and the relatively small number of extremely long careers can be explained by the "rich-get-richer" mechanism, which in this framework, provides more experienced and more reputable individuals with a competitive advantage in obtaining new career opportunities.
  • In his 2011 book Why Violence Has Declined, cognitive psychologist Steven Pinker refers to the Matthew Effect in societies, whereby everything seems to go right in some, and wrong in others. He speculates in Chapter 9 that this could be the result of a positive feedback loop in which reckless behavior by some individuals creates a chaotic environment that encourages reckless behavior by others. He cites research by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson showing that the more unstable the environment, the more steeply people discount the future, and thus the less forward-looking their behavior.
  • A large Matthew effect was discovered in a study of science funding in the Netherlands, where winners just above the funding threshold were found to accumulate more than twice as much funding during the subsequent eight years as non-winners with near-identical review scores that fell just below the threshold.[12]

In science, dramatic differences in the productivity may be explained by three phenomena: sacred spark, cumulative advantage, and search costs minimization by journal editors. The sacred spark paradigm suggests that scientists differ in their initial abilities, talent, skills, persistence, work habits, etc. that provide particular individuals with an early advantage. These factors have a multiplicative effect which helps these scholars succeed later. The cumulative advantage model argues that an initial success helps a researcher gain access to resources (e.g., teaching release, best graduate students, funding, facilities, etc.), which in turn results in further success. Search costs minimization by journal editors takes place when editors try to save time and effort by consciously or subconsciously selecting articles from well-known scholars. Whereas the exact mechanism underlying these phenomena is yet unknown, it is documented that a minority of all academics produce the most research output and attract the most citations.[13]

Education

In education, the term "Matthew effect" has been adopted by psychologist Keith Stanovich to describe a phenomenon observed in research on how new readers acquire the skills to read: early success in acquiring reading skills usually leads to later successes in reading as the learner grows, while failing to learn to read before the third or fourth year of schooling may be indicative of lifelong problems in learning new skills.

This is because children who fall behind in reading would read less, increasing the gap between them and their peers. Later, when students need to "read to learn" (where before they were learning to read), their reading difficulty creates difficulty in most other subjects. In this way they fall further and further behind in school, dropping out at a much higher rate than their peers.

In the words of Stanovich:

{{quote|Slow reading acquisition has cognitive, behavioral, and motivational consequences that slow the development of other cognitive skills and inhibit performance on many academic tasks. In short, as reading develops, other cognitive processes linked to it track the level of reading skill. Knowledge bases that are in reciprocal relationships with reading are also inhibited from further development. The longer this developmental sequence is allowed to continue, the more generalized the deficits will become, seeping into more and more areas of cognition and behavior. Or to put it more simply – and sadly – in the words of a tearful nine-year-old, already falling frustratingly behind his peers in reading progress, "Reading affects everything you do."[14]}}

Network science

In network science, the Matthew effect is used to describe the preferential attachment of earlier nodes in a network, which explains that these nodes tend to attract more links early on.[15] "Because of preferential attachment, a node that acquires more connections than another one will increase its connectivity at a higher rate, and thus an initial difference in the connectivity between two nodes will increase further as the network grows, while the degree of individual nodes will grow proportional with the square root of time."[16] The Matthew Effect therefore explains the growth of some nodes in vast networks such as the Internet.[17]

Markets with social influence

Product recommendations and information about past purchases have been shown{{by whom?|date=September 2018}} to influence consumers' choices significantly - whether for music, movie, book, technological, and other types of products. Social influence often induces a rich-get-richer phenomenon where popular products tend to become even more popular.[18]

See also

  • Capital accumulation
  • Google Scholar Effect
  • The internal contradictions of capital accumulation
  • Matilda effect
  • Pareto distribution
  • Positive feedback
  • Preferential attachment
  • {{section link|Quotation|Misquotation}}
  • Social network analysis
  • Virtuous circle and vicious circle
  • Wealth concentration

References

1. ^{{Cite book| edition = 1| publisher = Little, Brown and Company| isbn = 0-316-01792-2| last = Gladwell| first = Malcolm| title = Outliers: The Story of Success| date = 2008-11-18}}
2. ^{{cite web |accessdate=2009-01-12 |url=https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB122671469296530435 |title=The Elements of Success |work=The Wall Street Journal |date=2008-11-15 |author=Shaywitz, David A. }}
3. ^http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthewii.pdf
4. ^{{cite journal |author=Merton, Robert K. |year=1968 |url=http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthew1.pdf |title=The Matthew Effect in Science |journal=Science |volume=159 |issue=3810 |pages=56–63 |doi=10.1126/science.159.3810.56 |pmid=17737466|bibcode=1968Sci...159...56M }}
5. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Merton | first1 = Robert K | year = 1988 | title = The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative advantage and the symbolism of intellectual property | url = http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthewii.pdf| journal = ISIS | volume = 79 | issue = | pages = 606–623 | doi=10.1086/354848}}
6. ^Abstract of Merton's 1968 paper "The Matthew Effect in Science".
7. ^Salganik, Matthew J., Peter S. Dodds, and Duncan J. Watts. (2006). Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market (PDF). Science 311 , 854–856.
8. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Sorenson | first1 = Alan T | year = 2007 | title = Bestseller Lists and Product Variety | url = http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~sorensen/papers/sorensen_JIE_2007.pdf | format = PDF | journal = Journal of Industrial Economics | volume = 55 | issue = | pages = 715–738 }}
9. ^{{cite journal | last1 = van de Rijt | first1 = A. | last2 = Kang | first2 = S. | last3 = Restivo | first3 = M. | last4 = Patil | first4 = A. | year = 2014 | title = Field Experiments of Success-Breeds-Success Dynamics | url = http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/111/19/6934.full.pdf | format = PDF | journal = PNAS | volume = 111 | issue = 19 | pages = 6934–6939 }}
10. ^{{Cite book| edition = 2nd| publisher = Springer| isbn = 0-387-94868-6| last = Li| first = Ming|author2=Paul Vitanyi| title = An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications| date = 1997-02-27}}
11. ^{{Cite journal |last=Petersen |first=Alexander M. |author2=Jung, Woo-Sung |author3=Yang, Jae-Suk |author4=Stanley, H. Eugene |year= 2011 |title=Quantitative and Empirical demonstration of the Matthew Effect in a study of Career Longevity |journal=PNAS |volume=108 |issue=1 |pages=18–23 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1016733108 |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/108/1/18.short |quote= |pmid= 21173276|pmc= 3017158|postscript= |arxiv=0806.1224 |bibcode=2011PNAS..108...18P }}
12. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Bol | first1 = T. | last2 = de Vaan | first2 = M. | last3 = van de Rijt | first3 = A. | year = 2018 | title = The Matthew Effect in Science Funding | url = http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/115/19/4887.full.pdf | format = PDF | journal = PNAS | volume = 115 | issue = 19 | pages = 4887–4890 }}
13. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Serenko | first1 = A. | last2 = Cox | first2 = R. | last3 = Bontis | first3 = N. | last4 = Booker | first4 = L. | year = 2011 | title = The Superstar Phenomenon in the Knowledge Management and Intellectual Capital Academic Discipline | url = http://www.aserenko.com/papers/JOI_Serenko_Superstar_Phenomenon_Published.pdf | format = PDF | journal = Journal of Informetrics | volume = 5 | issue = | pages = 333–345 }}
14. ^Adams, Marilyn J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 59–60.
15. ^{{Cite journal|last=Barabási|first=A-L|year=1999|title=Emergence of scaling in random networks|journal=Science|volume=286|pages=509–512|doi=10.1126/science.286.5439.509|pmc=|pmid=10521342|quote=|postscript=|authorlink=|author2=Albert, R|accessdate=|arxiv=cond-mat/9910332|bibcode=1999Sci...286..509B}}
16. ^{{Cite journal|last=Perc|first=Matjaž|year=2014|title=The Matthew effect in empirical data|url=http://rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/11/98/20140378|journal=Interface|volume=12|issue=104|pages=20140378|doi=10.1098/rsif.2014.0378|pmc=|pmid=|quote=|postscript=|authorlink=|accessdate=|arxiv=1408.5124}}
17. ^{{Cite book|title=Networks, Complexity And Internet Regulation – Scale-Free Law|last=Guadamuz|first=Andres|date=2011|publisher=Edward Elgar|isbn=9781848443105}}
18. ^{{cite journal | url= http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0180040 | title= Transient dynamics in trial-offer markets with social influence: Trade-offs between appeal and quality | author1= Altszyler, E |author2= Berbeglia, F. | author3= Berbeglia, G. |author4= Van Hentenryck, P. | journal= PLoS ONE | year= 2017 |volume= 12 |issue= 7 | doi= 10.1371/journal.pone.0180040 | page= e0180040 |bibcode= 2017PLoSO..1280040A}}

Further reading

{{Refbegin|30em}}
  • {{cite journal | last1 = Bahr | first1 = Peter Riley | year = 2007 | title = Double jeopardy: Testing the effects of multiple basic skill deficiencies on successful remediation | url = | journal = Research in Higher Education | volume = 48 | issue = | pages = 695–725 | doi=10.1007/s11162-006-9047-y}}
  • Cunningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.
  • Rigney, Daniel (2010). The Matthew Effect: How Advantage Begets Further Advantage. Columbia University Press.
  • {{cite journal | last1 = Stanovich | first1 = Keith E | year = 1986 | title = Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy | url = http://www.psychologytoday.com/files/u81/Stanovich__1986_.pdf | format = PDF | journal = Reading Research Quarterly | volume = 21 | issue = 4| pages = 360–407 | doi=10.1598/rrq.21.4.1}}
  • Stanovich, Keith E. (2000). Progress in Understanding Reading: Scientific Foundations and New Frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.nningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.
{{refend}}{{DEFAULTSORT:Matthew Effect (sociology)}}

5 : Adages|Social phenomena|Sociological terminology|Words and phrases introduced in 1968|Gospel of Matthew

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/11 0:11:18