词条 | Motor learning |
释义 |
Motor learning is a change, resulting from practice or a novel experience, in the capability for responding. It often involves improving the smoothness and accuracy of movements and is obviously necessary for complicated movements such as speaking, playing the piano, and climbing trees; but it is also important for calibrating simple movements like reflexes, as parameters of the body and environment change over time. Motor learning research often considers variables that contribute to motor program formation (i.e., underlying skilled motor behaviour), sensitivity of error-detection processes,[1][2] and strength of movement schemas (see motor program). Motor learning is "relatively permanent", as the capability to respond appropriately is acquired and retained. As a result, the temporary processes that affect behaviour during practice or experience should not be considered learning, but rather transient performance effects. As such, the main components underlying the behavioural approach to motor learning are structure of practice and feedback given. The former pertains to the manipulation of timing and organization of practice (potentially for different subtasks or variations of the task) for optimal information retention (also see varied practice), while the latter pertains to the influence of feedback on the preparation, anticipation, and guidance of movement. Behavioural approachStructure of practice and contextual interferenceContextual interference was originally defined as "function interference in learning responsible for memory improvement".[3] Contextual interference effect is "the effect on learning of the degree of functional interference found in a practice situation when several tasks must be learned and are practiced together".[4] Variability of practice (or varied practice) is an important component to contextual interference, as it places task variations within learning. Although varied practice may lead to poor performance throughout the acquisition phase, it is important for the development of the schemata, which is responsible for the assembly and improved retention and transfer of motor learning.[3][5] Despite the improvements in performance seen across a range of studies, one limitation of the contextual interference effect is the uncertainty with regard to the cause of performance improvements as so many variables are constantly manipulated. In a review of literature,[3] the authors identify that there were few patterns to explain the improvements in experiments that use the contextual interference paradigm. Although there were no patterns in the literature, common areas and limitations that justified interference effects were identified:[3]
Feedback given during practiceFeedback is regarded as a critical variable for skill acquisition and is broadly defined as any kind of sensory information related to a response or movement.[7] Intrinsic feedback is response-produced — it occurs normally when a movement is made and the sources may be internal or external to the body. Typical sources of intrinsic feedback include vision, proprioception and audition. Extrinsic feedback is augmented information provided by an external source, in addition to intrinsic feedback. Extrinsic feedback is sometimes categorized as knowledge of performance or knowledge of results. Several studies have manipulated the presentation features of feedback information (e.g., frequency, delay, interpolated activities, and precision) in order to determine the optimal conditions for learning. See Figure 4, Figure 6, and summary Table 1[8] for a detailed explanation of feedback manipulation and knowledge of results (see below). Knowledge of performanceKnowledge of performance (KP) or kinematic feedback refers to information provided to a performer, indicating the quality or patterning of their movement.[7] It may include information such as displacement, velocity or joint motion. KP tends to be distinct from intrinsic feedback and more useful in real-world tasks. It is a strategy often employed by coaches or rehabilitation practitioners. Knowledge of resultsKnowledge of results (KR) is defined as extrinsic or augmented information provided to a performer after a response, indicating the success of their actions with regard to an environmental goal.[8] KR may be redundant with intrinsic feedback, especially in real-world scenarios.[7] However, in experimental studies, it refers to information provided over and above those sources of feedback that are naturally received when a response is made (i.e., response-produced feedback;[1][9][10] Typically, KR is also verbal or verbalizable.[19] The impact of KR on motor learning has been well-studied and some implications are described below. Experimental design and knowledge of resultsOften, experimenters fail to separate the relatively permanent aspect of change in the capability for responding (i.e. indicative of learning) from transient effects (i.e. indicative of performance). In order to account for this, transfer designs have been created which involve two distinct phases.[11] To visualize the transfer design, imagine a 4x4 grid. The column headings may be titled "Experiment #1" and "Experiment #2" and indicate the conditions you wish to compare. The row headings are titled "Acquisition" and "Transfer" whereby:
After a rest period, the change in the capability for responding (i.e. effects) are argued to be those attributed to learning, and the group with the most effective performance has learned the most. Functional role of knowledge of results and potential confounding of effectsKR seems to have many different roles, some of which can be viewed as temporary or transient (i.e. performance effects). Three of these roles include: 1) motivation, 2) associative function, and 3) guidance. The motivational influence can increase the effort and interest of the performer in the task as well as maintain this interest once KR is removed.[12] Though important to create interest in the task for performance and learning purposes, however the extent to which it affects learning is unknown. The associative function of KR is likely to be involved in the formation of associations between stimulus and response (i.e., Law of Effect).[13] However, this additional effect is not able to account for findings in transfer tasks manipulating the relative frequency of KR; specifically, decreasing relative frequency results in enhanced learning. For an alternate discussion on how KR may calibrate the motor system to the outside world (see schema theory in motor program). The guidance role of KR is likely the most influential to learning[1] as both internal and external sources of feedback play a guiding role in performance of a motor task. As the performer is informed of errors in task performance, the discrepancy can be used to continually improve performance in following trials. However, the guidance hypothesis postulates that provision of too much external, augmented feedback (e.g., KR) during practice may cause the learner to develop a harmful dependency on this source of feedback.[8] This may lead to superior performance during practice but poor performance at transfer – an indication of poor motor learning. Additionally, it implies that, as the performer improves, the conditions of KR must be adapted according to the performer’s skill and difficulty of the task in order to maximize learning (see challenge point framework). Specificity of learning hypothesisThe specificity of learning hypothesis suggests that learning is most effective when practice sessions include environment and movement conditions which closely resemble those required during performance of the task — replicating the target skill level and context for performance.[7]p. 194 It suggests that the benefit of specificity in practice occurs because motor learning is combined with physical practice during the learned sport or skill.[14]p. 90 Contrary to previous beliefs, skill learning is accomplished by alternating motor learning and physical performance, making the sources of feedback work together. The learning process, especially for a difficult task, results in the creation of a representation of the task where all relevant information pertaining to task performance is integrated. This representation becomes tightly coupled with increasing experience performing the task. As a result, removing or adding a significant source of information after a practice period where it was present or not, does not cause performance to deteriorate. Alternating motor learning and physical practice can ultimately lead to a great, if not better performance as opposed to just physical practice. Physiological approachThe cerebellum and basal ganglia are critical for motor learning. As a result of the universal need for properly calibrated movement, it is not surprising that the cerebellum and basal ganglia are widely conserved across vertebrates from fish to humans.[15] Through motor learning the human is capable of achieving very skilled behavior, and through repetitive training a degree of automaticity can be expected. And although this can be a refined process much has been learned from studies of simple behaviors. These behaviors include eyeblink conditioning, motor learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex, and birdsong. Research on Aplysia californica, the sea slug, has yielded detailed knowledge of the cellular mechanisms of a simple form of learning. A type of motor learning occurs during operation of a brain-computer interface. For example, Mikhail Lebedev, Miguel Nicolelis and their colleagues recently demonstrated cortical plasticity that resulted in incorporation of an external actuator controlled through a brain-machine interface into the subject's neural representation.[16] At a cellular level, motor learning manifests itself in the neurons of the motor cortex. Using single-cell recording techniques, Dr. Emilio Bizzi and his collaborators have shown the behavior of certain cells, known as "memory cells," can undergo lasting alteration with practice. Motor learning is also accomplished on the musculoskeletal level. Each motor neuron in the body innervates one or more muscle cells, and together these cells form what is known as a motor unit. For a person to perform even the simplest motor task, the activity of thousands of these motor units must be coordinated. It appears that the body handles this challenge by organizing motor units into modules of units whose activity is correlated.{{Citation needed|date=October 2010}} Disordered motor learningDevelopmental coordination disorderImpairments associated with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) involve difficulty in learning new motor skills as well as limited postural control and deficits in sensorimotor coordination.[17] It appears that children with DCD are not able to improve performance of complex motor tasks by practice alone.[18] However, there is evidence that task-specific training can improve performance of simpler tasks.[19] Impaired skills learning may be correlated with brain activity, particularly, a reduction of brain activity in regions associated with skilled motor practice.[20] ApraxiaMotor learning has been applied to stroke recovery and neurorehabilitation, as rehabilitation is generally a process of relearning lost skills through practice and/or training.[21] Although rehabilitation clinicians utilize practice as a major component within an intervention, a gap remains between motor control and motor learning research and rehabilitation practice. Common motor learning paradigms include robot arm paradigms, where individuals are encouraged to resist against a hand held device throughout specific arm movements. Another important concept to motor learning is the amount practice implemented in an intervention. Studies regarding the relationship between the amount of training received and the retention of the memory a set amount of time afterwards have been a popular focus in research. It has been shown that over learning leads to major improvements in long term retention and little effect on performance.[22] Motor learning practice paradigms have compared the differences of different practice schedules, and it has proposed that repetition of the same movements is not enough in order to relearn a skill, as it is unclear whether true brain recovery is elicited through repetition alone.[21] It is suggested that compensation methods develop through pure repetition and to elicit cortical changes (true recovery), individuals should be exposed to more challenging tasks. Research that has implemented motor learning and rehabilitation practice has been used within the stroke population and includes arm ability training, constraint-induced movement therapy, electromyograph-triggered neuromuscular stimulation, interactive robot therapy and virtual reality-based rehabilitation. A recent study ischemic conditioning was delivered via blood pressure cuff inflation and deflation to the arm, to facilitate learning. It showed for the first time in humans and animals, that ischemic conditioning can enhance motor learning and that the enhancement is retained over time. The potential benefits of ischemic conditioning extend far beyond stroke to other neuro-, geriatric, and pediatric rehabilitation populations.[23] These findings were featured on Global Medical Discovery news.[24] See also
References1. ^1 2 {{cite journal |author=Adams JA |title=A closed-loop theory of motor learning |journal=J mot Behav |volume=3 |issue=2 |pages=111–49 |date=June 1971 |pmid=15155169 |doi=10.1080/00222895.1971.10734898 |url=}} 2. ^{{cite journal| last1=Schmidt| first1=Richard A.|title=A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning.| journal=Psychological Review|volume=82| issue=4|year=1975| pages=225–260| url=http://www.hp-research.com/sites/default/files/publications/Schmidt%20%28Psych%20Review,%201975%29.pdf|doi=10.1037/h0076770}} 3. ^1 2 3 4 {{cite journal|last1=Barreiros| first1=J.| last2=Figueiredo| first2=T.|last3=Godinho| first3=M.| title=The contextual interference effect in applied settings|journal=European Physical Education Review|volume=13| issue=2|year=2007| pages=195–208| issn=1356-336X|doi=10.1177/1356336X07076876}} 4. ^{{cite journal|last1=Magill|first1=Richard A.|last2=Hall|first2=Kellie G.|title=A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition|journal=Human Movement Science|volume=9|issue=3–5| year=1990|pages=241–289|doi=10.1016/0167-9457(90)90005-X}} 5. ^{{cite journal |author=Moxley SE |title=Schema: the variability of practice hypothesis |journal=J mot Behav |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=65–70 |date=January 1979 |pmid=15186973 |doi=10.1080/00222895.1979.10735173 |url=}} 6. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Smith PJ, Davies M |title=Applying contextual interference to the Pawlata roll |journal=J Sports Sci |volume=13 |issue=6 |pages=455–62 |date=December 1995 |pmid=8850571 |doi=10.1080/02640419508732262 |url=}} 7. ^1 2 3 {{Cite book | last1 = Schmidt | first1 = Richard A. | last2 = Wrisberg | first2 = Craig A. | title = Motor learning and performance | year = 2004 | publisher = Human Kinetics | location = Champaign, IL | isbn = 978-0-7360-4566-7 | oclc = 474742713 }} 8. ^1 2 {{cite journal |vauthors=Salmoni AW, Schmidt RA, Walter CB |title=Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal |journal=Psychol Bull |volume=95 |issue=3 |pages=355–86 |date=May 1984 |pmid=6399752 |doi= 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.355|url=}} 9. ^{{cite book | last1 = James | first1 = William | title = Principles of psychology. | publisher = Dover Publications | date = 1890-c1918 | year = 1950 | location = New York | isbn = 9780486203812| oclc = 191755}} 10. ^{{cite journal|last1=Adams|first1=Jack A.|title=Response feedback and learning.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|volume=70|issue=6, Pt.1|year=1968| pages=486–504| doi=10.1037/h0026741}} 11. ^1 {{Cite book | last1 = Schmidt | first1 = Richard A. | last2 = Lee | first2 = Timothy Donald | title = Motor control and learning : a behavioral emphasis | year = 2005 | publisher = Human Kinetics | location = Champaign, IL | isbn = 978-0-7360-4258-1 | oclc = 265658315 }} 12. ^{{cite journal|last1=Elwell|first1=J. L.|last2=Grindley|first2=G. C.|title=THE EFFECT OF KNOWLEDGE OF RESULTS ON LEARNING AND PERFORMANCE|journal=British Journal of Psychology. General Section|volume=29| issue=1|year=1938|pages=39–54|doi=10.1111/j.2044-8295.1938.tb00899.x}} 13. ^{{cite journal |author=Nevin J |title=Analyzing Thorndike's Law Of Effect: The Question Of Stimulus-response Bonds |journal=J Exp Anal Behav |volume=72 |issue=3 |pages=447–50 |date=November 1999 |pmid=16812923 |pmc=1284755 |doi= 10.1901/jeab.1999.72-447|url=}} 14. ^{{cite book | last1 = Proteau | first1 = Luc | title = On the Specificity of Learning and the Role of Visual Information for Movement Control | editors = L Proteau; D Elliott | work = Vision and Motor Control. Advances in Psychology, Volume 85 |pages = 33–48| publisher = Elsevier Science & Technology |location = New York | year = 1992 | isbn = 9781281789396 |oclc = 742292994}} 15. ^{{Cite journal|title = The evolutionary origin of the vertebrate basal ganglia and its role in action selection|last = Grillner|first = Sten|date = 2013|journal = Journal of Physiology|doi = 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.246660|pmid = 23318875|last2 = Robertson|first2 = Brita|last3 = Stephenson-Jones|first3 = Marcus|volume=591|issue = 22|pages=5425–31|pmc=3853485}} 16. ^{{cite journal |author=Lebedev MA, Carmena JM, O'Doherty JE, Nicolelis, MAL|title=Cortical ensemble adaptation to represent velocity of an artificial actuator controlled by a brain-machine interface |journal=J. Neurosci. |volume=25 |issue=19 |pages=4681–93 |date=May 2005 |pmid=15888644 |doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4088-04.2005 |url=|display-authors=etal}} 17. ^{{cite journal |author=Geuze RH |title=Postural control in children with developmental coordination disorder |journal=Neural Plast. |volume=12 |issue=2–3 |pages=183–96; discussion 263–72 |year=2005 |pmid=16097486 |pmc=2565450 |doi=10.1155/NP.2005.183 |url=}} 18. ^{{cite journal | title = Kinematic analysis of skill acquisition in physically awkward boys. | journal = Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly | date = October 1987 | first = Gordon E | last = Marchiori |author2=Albert E. Wall |author3=Wendy Bedingfield | volume = 4 | issue = 4 | pages = 305–315| id = | url = http://journals.humankinetics.com/apaq-back-issues/APAQVolume4Issue4October/KinematicAnalysisofSkillAcquisitioninPhysicallyAwkwardBoys | accessdate = 2013-12-02| doi = 10.1123/apaq.4.4.305 }} 19. ^{{cite journal | title = Task-Specific Intervention With Children Reduces Movement Problems | journal = Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly | date = 1993-01-01 | first = Gay | last = Revie |author2=Dawne Larkin | volume = 10 | issue = 1 | pages = 29–41| id = | url = http://journals.humankinetics.com/AcuCustom/Sitename/Documents/DocumentItem/12590.pdf | accessdate = 2013-12-02| doi = 10.1123/apaq.10.1.29 }} 20. ^{{cite journal |vauthors=Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, Boyd LA |title=Brain activation associated with motor skill practice in children with developmental coordination disorder: an fMRI study |journal=Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. |volume=29 |issue=2 |pages=145–52 |date=April 2011 |pmid=21145385 |doi=10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2010.12.002 |url=}} 21. ^1 {{cite journal |author=Krakauer JW |title=Motor learning: its relevance to stroke recovery and neurorehabilitation |journal=Curr. Opin. Neurol. |volume=19 |issue=1 |pages=84–90 |date=February 2006 |pmid=16415682 |url=http://columbiampl.org/pdf/Articles/B_Motor%20Learning.pdf |doi=10.1097/01.wco.0000200544.29915.cc}} 22. ^{{cite journal|last=Joiner|first=Wilsaan|author2=Smith, Maurice|title=Long-Term Retention Explained by a Model of Short-Term Learning in the Adaptive Control of Reaching|journal=J Neurophysiol|date=September 2008|volume=100|issue=5|pages=2848–2955|doi=10.1152/jn.90706.2008|pmid=18784273|pmc=2585394}} 23. ^{{Cite journal|title = Remote limb ischemic conditioning enhances motor learning in healthy humans|journal = Journal of Neurophysiology|date = 2015-06-01|issn = 0022-3077|pmc = 4468973|pmid = 25867743|pages = 3708–3719|volume = 113|issue = 10|doi = 10.1152/jn.01028.2014|first = Kendra M.|last = Cherry-Allen|first2 = Jeff M.|last2 = Gidday|first3 = Jin-Moo|last3 = Lee|first4 = Tamara|last4 = Hershey|first5 = Catherine E.|last5 = Lang}} 24. ^{{Cite web|title = Remote limb ischemic conditioning enhances motor learning in healthy humans.|url = https://globalmedicaldiscovery.com/key-clinical-research-articles-global-medica-discovery/remote-limb-ischemic-conditioning-enhances-motor-learning-in-healthy-humans/|website = globalmedicaldiscovery.com|accessdate = 2015-09-27}} Further reading
External links
4 : Memory processes|Motor skills|Nervous system|Motor control |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。