词条 | Neuropsychology |
释义 |
In practice, neuropsychologists tend to work in research settings (universities, laboratories or research institutions), clinical settings (medical hospitals or rehabilitation settings, often involved in assessing or treating patients with neuropsychological problems), or forensic settings or industry (often as clinical-trial consultants where CNS function is a concern). HistoryNeuropsychology is a relatively new discipline within the field of psychology. The first textbook defining the field, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology, was initially published by Kolb and Whishaw in 1980.[3] However, the history of its development can be traced back to the Third Dynasty in ancient Egypt, perhaps even earlier.[4] There is much debate as to when societies started considering the functions of different organs. For many centuries, the brain was thought useless and was often discarded during burial processes and autopsies. As the field of medicine developed its understanding of human anatomy and physiology, different theories were developed as to why the body functioned the way it did. Many times, bodily functions were approached from a religious point of view and abnormalities were blamed on bad spirits and the gods. The brain has not always been considered the center of the functioning body. It has taken hundreds of years to develop our understanding of the brain and how it affects our behaviors. Ancient EgyptIn ancient Egypt, writings on medicine date from the time of the priest Imhotep.[5] They took a more scientific approach to medicine and disease, describing the brain, trauma, abnormalities, and remedies for reference for future physicians. Despite this, Egyptians saw the heart not the brain as the seat of the soul.[6] AristotleAristotle reinforced this focus on the heart which originated in Egypt. He believed the heart to be in control of mental processes, and looked on the brain, due to its inert nature, as a mechanism for cooling the heat generated by the heart.[8][9] He drew his conclusions based on the empirical study of animals. He found that while their brains were cold to the touch and that such contact did not trigger any movements, the heart was warm and active, accelerating and slowing dependent on mood.[8][9] Such beliefs were upheld by many for years to come, persisting through the Middle Ages and the Renaissance period until they began to falter in the 17th Century due to further research.[9] The influence of Aristotle in the development of neuropsychology is evident within language used in modern day, since we "follow our hearts" and "learn by the heart".[9] {{main|Aristotle#Thought}} HippocratesHippocrates looked upon the brain as the seat of the soul. He drew a connection between the brain and behaviors of the body saying "The brain exercises the greatest power in the man".[10] Apart from moving the focus from the heart as the "seat of the soul" to the brain, Hippocrates did not go into much detail about its actual functioning. However, by switching the attention of the medical community to the brain, the doors were opened to a more scientific discovery of the organ responsible for our behaviors. For years to come, scientists were inspired to explore the functions of the body and to find concrete explanations for both normal and abnormal behaviors. Scientific discovery led them to believe that there were natural and organically occurring reasons to explain various functions of the body, and it could all be traced back to the brain. Over the years, science would continue to expand and the mysteries of the world would begin to make sense, or at least be looked at in a different way. Hippocrates introduced man to the concept of the mind – which was widely seen as a separate function apart from the actual brain organ. René DescartesPhilosopher René Descartes expanded upon this idea and is most widely known by his work on the mind-body problem. Often, Descartes' ideas were looked upon as overly philosophical and lacking in sufficient scientific background. Descartes focused much of his anatomical experimentation on the brain, paying specific attention to the pineal gland – which he argued was the actual "seat of the soul". Still deeply rooted in a spiritual outlook towards the scientific world, the body was said to be mortal, and the soul immortal. The pineal gland was then thought to be the very place at which the mind would interact with the mortal and machine-like body. At the time, Descartes was convinced the mind had control over the behaviors of the body (controlling the man) – but also that the body could have influence over the mind, which is referred to as dualism.[11] This idea that the mind essentially had control over the body, but man's body could resist or even influence other behaviors was a major turning point in the way many physiologists would look at the brain. The capabilities of the mind were observed to do much more than simply react, but also to be rational and function in organized, thoughtful ways – much more complex than he thought the animal world to be. These ideas, although disregarded by many and cast aside for years led the medical community to expand their own ideas of the brain and begin to understand in new ways just how intricate the workings of the brain really were, and the complete effects it had on daily life, as well, which treatments would be the most beneficial to helping those people living with a dysfunctional mind. The mind-body problem, spurred by René Descartes, continues to this day with many philosophical arguments both for and against his ideas. However controversial they were and remain today, the fresh and well-thought-out perspective Descartes presented has had long-lasting effects on the various disciplines of medicine, psychology and much more, especially in putting an emphasis on separating the mind from the body in order to explain observable behaviors. Thomas WillisIt was in the mid-17th century that another major contributor to the field of neuropsychology emerged. Thomas Willis studied at Oxford University and took a physiological approach to the brain and behavior. It was Willis who coined the words 'hemisphere' and 'lobe' when referring to the brain.[12] He was one of the earliest to use the words 'neurology' and 'psychology'. Rejecting the idea that humans were the only beings capable of rational thought, Willis looked at specialized structures of the brain.[9] He theorized that higher structures accounted for complex functions, whereas lower structures were responsible for functions similar to those seen in other animals, consisting mostly of reactions and automatic responses.[13] He was particularly interested in people who suffered from manic disorders and hysteria.[14][15] His research constituted some of the first times that psychiatry and neurology came together to study individuals. Through his in-depth study of the brain and behavior, Willis concluded that automated responses such as breathing, heartbeats and other various motor activities were carried out within the lower region of the brain. Although much of his work has been made obsolete, his ideas presented the brain as more complex than previously imagined, and led the way for future pioneers to understand and build upon his theories, especially when it came to looking at disorders and dysfunctions in the brain.[14] Franz Joseph GallNeuroanatomist and physiologist Franz Joseph Gall made major progress in understanding the brain. He theorized that personality was directly related to features and structures within the brain. However, Gall's major contribution within the field of neuroscience is his invention of phrenology. This new discipline looked at the brain as an organ of the mind, where the shape of the skull could ultimately determine one's intelligence and personality.[16] This theory was like many circulating at the time, as many scientists were taking into account physical features of the face and body, head size, anatomical structure, and levels of intelligence; only Gall looked primarily at the brain. There was much debate over the validity of Gall's claims however, because he was often found to be wrong in his predictions. He was once sent a cast of René Descartes' skull, and through his method of phrenology claimed the subject must have had a limited capacity for reasoning and higher cognition.[17] As controversial and false as many of Gall's claims were, his contributions to understanding cortical regions of the brain and localized activity continued to advance understanding of the brain, personality, and behavior. His work is considered crucial to having laid a firm foundation in the field of neuropsychology, which would flourish over the next few decades. Jean-Baptiste BouillaudTowards the late 19th century, the belief that the size of ones skull could determine their level of intelligence was discarded as science and medicine moved forward. A physician by the name of Jean-Baptiste Bouillaud expanded upon the ideas of Gall and took a closer look at the idea of distinct cortical regions of the brain each having their own independent function. Bouillaud was specifically interested in speech and wrote many publications on the anterior region of the brain being responsible for carrying out the act of ones speech, a discovery that had stemmed from the research of Gall. He was also one of the first to use larger samples for research although it took many years for that method to be accepted. By looking at over a hundred different case studies, Bouillaud came to discover that it was through different areas of the brain that speech is completed and understood. By observing people with brain damage, his theory was made more concrete. Bouillaud, along with many other pioneers of the time made great advances within the field of neurology, especially when it came to localization of function. There are many arguable debates as to who deserves the most credit for such discoveries,[18] and often, people remain unmentioned, but Paul Broca is perhaps one of the most famous and well known contributors to neuropsychology – often referred to as "the father" of the discipline. Paul BrocaInspired by the advances being made in the area of localized function within the brain, Paul Broca committed much of his study to the phenomena of how speech is understood and produced. Through his study, it was discovered and expanded upon that we articulate via the left hemisphere. Broca's observations and methods are widely considered to be where neuropsychology really takes form as a recognizable and respected discipline. Armed with the understanding that specific, independent areas of the brain are responsible for articulation and understanding of speech, the brains abilities were finally being acknowledged as the complex and highly intricate organ that it is. Broca was essentially the first to fully break away from the ideas of phrenology and delve deeper into a more scientific and psychological view of the brain.[19] Karl Spencer Lashley{{Main|Karl Lashley}}Lashley's works and theories that follow are summarized in his book Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence.[20] Lashley's theory of the Engram was the driving force for much of his research. An engram was believed to be a part of the brain where a specific memory was stored. He continued to use the training/ablation method that Franz had taught him. He would train a rat to learn a maze and then use systematic lesions and removed sections of cortical tissue to see if the rat forgot what it had learned. Through his research with the rats, he learned that forgetting was dependent on the amount of tissue removed and not where it was removed from. He called this mass action and he believed that it was a general rule that governed how brain tissue would respond, independent of the type of learning. But we know now that mass action was true for these rats, because learning to run a maze is known as complex learning and it requires multiple cortical areas, so cutting into individual parts alone will not erase the memory from the rats' brains, but taking large sections removes multiple cortical areas at one time and so they can forget. Lashley also discovered that a portion of a functional area could carry out the role of the entire area, even when the rest of the area has been removed. He called this phenomenon equipotentiality. We know now that he was seeing evidence of plasticity in the brain. The brain has the spectacular ability for certain areas to take over the functions of other areas if those areas should fail or be removed. Approaches{{unreferenced section|date=June 2013}}Experimental neuropsychology is an approach that uses methods from experimental psychology to uncover the relationship between the nervous system and cognitive function. The majority of work involves studying healthy humans in a laboratory setting, although a minority of researchers may conduct animal experiments. Human work in this area often takes advantage of specific features of our nervous system (for example that visual information presented to a specific visual field is preferentially processed by the cortical hemisphere on the opposite side) to make links between neuroanatomy and psychological function.[21] Clinical neuropsychology is the application of neuropsychological knowledge to the assessment (see neuropsychological test and neuropsychological assessment), management, and rehabilitation of people who have suffered illness or injury (particularly to the brain) which has caused neurocognitive problems. In particular they bring a psychological viewpoint to treatment, to understand how such illness and injury may affect and be affected by psychological factors.[22] They also can offer an opinion as to whether a person is demonstrating difficulties due to brain pathology or as a consequence of an emotional or another (potentially) reversible cause or both. For example, a test might show that both patients X and Y are unable to name items that they have been previously exposed to within the past 20 minutes (indicating possible dementia). If patient Y can name some of them with further prompting (e.g. given a categorical clue such as being told that the item they could not name is a fruit), this allows a more specific diagnosis than simply dementia (Y appears to have the vascular type which is due to brain pathology but is usually at least somewhat reversible). Clinical neuropsychologists often work in hospital settings in an interdisciplinary medical team; others work in private practice and may provide expert input into medico-legal proceedings.[23]Cognitive neuropsychology is a relatively new development and has emerged as a distillation of the complementary approaches of both experimental and clinical neuropsychology. It seeks to understand the mind and brain by studying people who have suffered brain injury or neurological illness. One model of neuropsychological functioning is known as functional localization.[24] This is based on the principle that if a specific cognitive problem can be found after an injury to a specific area of the brain, it is possible that this part of the brain is in some way involved. However, there may be reason to believe that the link between mental functions and neural regions is not so simple. An alternative model of the link between mind and brain, such as parallel processing, may have more explanatory power for the workings and dysfunction of the human brain. Yet another approach investigates how the pattern of errors produced by brain-damaged individuals can constrain our understanding of mental representations and processes without reference to the underlying neural structure. A more recent but related approach is cognitive neuropsychiatry which seeks to understand the normal function of mind and brain by studying psychiatric or mental illness.[25]Connectionism is the use of artificial neural networks to model specific cognitive processes using what are considered to be simplified but plausible models of how neurons operate. Once trained to perform a specific cognitive task these networks are often damaged or 'lesioned' to simulate brain injury or impairment in an attempt to understand and compare the results to the effects of brain injury in humans.[26]Functional neuroimaging uses specific neuroimaging technologies to take readings from the brain, usually when a person is doing a particular task, in an attempt to understand how the activation of particular brain areas is related to the task. In particular, the growth of methodologies to employ cognitive testing within established functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques to study brain-behavior relations is having a notable influence on neuropsychological research.[27]In practice these approaches are not mutually exclusive and most neuropsychologists select the best approach or approaches for the task to be completed. Methods and tools
See also{{Portal|Thinking|Neuroscience|Psychology|Mind and Brain}}{{col div|colwidth=20em}}
References1. ^{{Cite book|title=Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 4th Edition|last=Gazzaniga|first=|publisher=|year=2013|isbn=978-0393913484|location=|pages=}} 2. ^{{Cite journal | last1 = Posner | first1 = M. I. | last2 = Digirolamo | first2 = G. J. | doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.873 | title = Cognitive neuroscience: Origins and promise | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 126 | issue = 6 | pages = 873–889 | year = 2000 | pmid = 11107880| pmc = }} 3. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.psych.ualberta.ca/GCPWS/KolbWhishaw/KolbBio/Kolb_bio1.html|publisher=University of Calgary|accessdate=14 August 2017|title=The Great Canadian Psychology Website - Researchers}} 4. ^{{cite book |last=Finger |first=Stanley |title=Minds Behind the Brain: A History of the Pioneers and their discoveries |year=2000 |publisher=Oxford |location=New York |isbn=978-0-19-518182-1 |pages=22 |ref=harv}} 5. ^{{cite news|last1=Highfield|first1=Roger|title=How Imhotep gave us medicine|url=https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/science/science-news/3293164/How-Imhotep-gave-us-medicine.html|accessdate=24 March 2018|work=The Daily Telegraph}} 6. ^{{Cite journal|last=Carus|first=Paul|date=1905|title=The Conception of the Soul and the Belief in Resurrection Among the Egyptians|journal=The Monist|volume=15|issue=3|pages=409–428|jstor=27899609|doi=10.5840/monist190515326}} 7. ^{{cite journal |last=Warren |first=Howard |url=https://books.google.com/?id=D4IXAAAAYAAJ&pg=PA19 |title=A History of the Association Psychology |journal=Nature |volume=110 |issue=2750 |date=1921 |pages=19–30, 259, 296|bibcode=1922Natur.110S..75. |doi=10.1038/110075d0 }} 8. ^1 {{Cite web|url=https://brainaacn.org/history-of-neuropsychology/|title=History of Neuropsychology {{!}} BRAIN|website=brainaacn.org|language=en-US|access-date=2018-09-25}} 9. ^1 2 3 4 {{Cite journal|last=Benton|first=A.L.|last2=Sivan|first2=Abigail|date=2007-04-01|title=Clinical Neuropsychology: A Brief History|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/6291178|journal=Disease-a-month : DM|volume=53|issue=3|pages=142–7|doi=10.1016/j.disamonth.2007.04.003|pmid=17544643}} 10. ^{{harvnb|Finger|2000|p=44}} 11. ^{{harvnb|Finger|2000||p=92}} 12. ^{{Citation|last=Finger|first=Stanley|date=1994|pages=1–28|publisher=Elsevier|doi=10.1016/b978-0-08-092668-1.50007-7|isbn=9780080926681|title=Neuropsychology|chapter=History of Neuropsychology}} 13. ^{{Citation|last=Finger|first=Stanley|date=2005-03-03|pages=85–100|publisher=Oxford University Press|language=en-US|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195181821.003.0007|isbn=9780195181821|title=Minds Behind the Brain|chapter=Thomas Willis: The Functional Organization of the Brain}} 14. ^1 {{Cite journal|last=Arráez-Aybar|first=Luis-Alfonso|last2=Navia-Álvarez|first2=Pedro|last3=Fuentes-Redondo|first3=Talia|last4=Bueno-López|first4=José-L|date=March 2015|title=Thomas Willis, a pioneer in translational research in anatomy (on the 350th anniversary of Cerebri anatome)|journal=Journal of Anatomy|volume=226|issue=3|pages=289–300|doi=10.1111/joa.12273|issn=0021-8782|pmc=4337668|pmid=25688933}} 15. ^{{Cite journal|last=Eadie|first=M. J.|date=March 2003|title=A pathology of the animal spirits -- the clinical neurology of Thomas Willis (1621-1675). Part II -- disorders of intrinsically abnormal animal spirits|journal=Journal of Clinical Neuroscience|volume=10|issue=2|pages=146–157|issn=0967-5868|pmid=12637040|doi=10.1016/S0967-5868(02)00164-9}} 16. ^{{cite book|last=Benton|first=Arthur|title=History of Neuropsychology: Selected Papers|year=2000|publisher=Oxford|location=USA}} 17. ^{{harvnb|Finger|2000|p=151}} 18. ^{{cite book|last=Viney|first=Wayne|title=A History of Psychology: Ideas and Context|edition=3rd|year=2003|publisher=Pearson|location=Boston}} 19. ^{{Cite journal | last1 = Cubelli | first1 = R. | last2 = De Bastiani | first2 = P. | doi = 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.004 | title = 150 Years after Leborgne: Why is Paul Broca so important in the history of neuropsychology? | journal = Cortex | volume = 47 | issue = 2 | pages = 146–147 | year = 2011 | pmid = 21112584| pmc = }} 20. ^{{Cite journal | last1 = Carmichael | first1 = L. | title = Karl Spencer Lashley, Experimental Psychologist | doi = 10.1126/science.129.3360.1410 | journal = Science | volume = 129 | issue = 3360 | pages = 1410–1412 | year = 1959 | pmid = 13658968| pmc = | bibcode = 1959Sci...129.1410C }} 21. ^{{Cite web|url=https://www.allpsychologycareers.com/topics/experimental-neuropsychology.html|title=What is Experimental Neuropsychology?|website=www.allpsychologycareers.com|access-date=2018-09-25}} 22. ^{{Cite news|url=https://www.ukessays.com/essays/psychology/evaluating-the-relationship-between-clinical-and-experimental-neuropsychology-psychology-essay.php|title=Clinical and experimental neuropsychology|work=UKEssays|access-date=2018-09-25|language=en-GB}} 23. ^{{Cite web|url=http://drdougcohen.com/index.html|title=Neuropsychology|last=Cohen|first=Dr. Douglas|date=2008|website=Dr Doug Cohen|archive-url=|archive-date=|dead-url=|access-date=}} 24. ^{{Citation|last=Stebbins|first=Glenn T.|date=2007|pages=539–557|publisher=Elsevier|doi=10.1016/b978-141603618-0.10027-x|isbn=9781416036180|title=Textbook of Clinical Neurology|chapter=Neuropsychological Testing}} 25. ^{{Citation|last=Hall|first=Jeremy|title=Neuropsychology|date=2010|work=Companion to Psychiatric Studies|pages=121–140|publisher=Elsevier|doi=10.1016/b978-0-7020-3137-3.00007-3|isbn=9780702031373|last2=O'Carroll|first2=Ronan E|last3=Frith|first3=Chris D}} 26. ^{{Citation|last=Garson|first=James|title=Connectionism|date=2018|url=https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2018/entries/connectionism/|work=The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy|editor-last=Zalta|editor-first=Edward N.|edition=Fall 2018|publisher=Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University|access-date=2018-09-25}} 27. ^{{Cite web|url=http://www.imedpub.com/journal-cognitive-neuropsychology/|title=Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychology {{!}} Peer Reviewed Journal|website=www.imedpub.com|language=en|access-date=2018-09-25}} 28. ^Boyle, G.J., Saklofske, D.H., & Matthews, G. (2012). (Eds.), SAGE Benchmarks in Psychology: Psychological Assessment, Vol. 3: Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment. London: SAGE. {{ISBN|978-0-85702-270-7}} 29. ^{{cite journal|url=http://www.cell.com/neuron/abstract/S0896-6273(16)30781-4?_returnURL=http%3A%2F%2Flinkinghub.elsevier.com%2Fretrieve%2Fpii%2FS0896627316307814%3Fshowall%3Dtrue|journal=Neuron|date=7 December 2016|author=Xiao-Jing Wang|author2=Wei Wei (New York University Global Brain Project)|title=Inhibitory Control in the Cortico-Basal Ganglia-Thalamocortical Loop: Complex Regulation and Interplay with Memory and Decision Processes|doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.031|pmid=27866799|pmc=5193098|volume=92|issue=5|pages=1093–1105}} 30. ^{{Cite journal |author=Bauer, R. M.|display-authors=etal|title=Computerized Neuropsychological Assessment Devices: Joint Position Paper of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology |journal=Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology |volume=27 |issue=3 |date=May 2012 |doi=10.1093/arclin/acs027 |pmid=22382386 |pages=362–373|pmc=3499090 }} 31. ^{{cite web|url=http://medicinex.stanford.edu/conf/submission/view/343|author=Stanford|author-link=Stanford|date=6 July 2015|title=Neuropsychology based Behavior Design through Mobile Health Gaming|publisher=Stanford University School of Medicine}} 32. ^{{cite web|url=http://2014.obesityweek.com/wp/uploads/2014/10/TOS-Posters.pdf|publisher=The Obesity Society|author=Dr Craig Johnston|author2=Dr Jennette Moreno|display-authors=etal|title=A Nutrition Education Mobile Game Impacts Snack Selection in Middle School Students|date=2 November 2014|deadurl=yes|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20160304060846/http://2014.obesityweek.com/wp/uploads/2014/10/TOS-Posters.pdf|archivedate=4 March 2016|df=}} External links{{Library resources box |by=no |onlinebooks=no |others=no |about=yes |label=Neuropsychology}}{{Footer Neuropsychology}}{{Psychology}}{{Neuroscience}}{{Authority control}} 1 : Neuropsychology |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。