词条 | Biological basis of personality | |||||||||||||||||
释义 |
The biological basis of personality is the collection of brain systems and mechanisms that underlie human personality. Human neurobiology, especially as it relates to complex traits and behaviors, is not well understood, but research into the neuroanatomical and functional underpinnings of personality are an active field of research. Animal models of behavior, molecular biology, and brain imaging techniques have provided some insight into human personality, especially trait theories. Much of the current understanding of personality from a neurobiological perspective places an emphasis on the biochemistry of the behavioral systems of reward, motivation, and punishment. This has led to a few biologically based personality theories such as Eysenck's three factor model of personality, Grey's reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST), and Cloninger's model of personality. The Big Five model of personality is not biologically based; yet some research in the differences in brain structures provided biological support also for this model. Defining personality in a biological contextPersonality can be defined as a set of characteristics or traits that drive individual differences in human behavior. From a biological perspective, these traits can be traced back to brain structures and neural mechanisms. However, this definition and theory of biological basis is not universally accepted. There are many conflicting theories of personality in the fields of psychology, psychiatry, philosophy, and neuroscience. A few examples of this are the nature vs. nurture debate and how the idea of a 'soul' fits into biological theories of personality.[1]History of biology-based personality researchSince the time of the ancient Greeks, humankind has attempted to explain personality through spiritual beliefs, philosophy, and psychology. Historically, studies of personality have traditionally come from the social sciences and humanities, but in the past two decades neuroscience has begun to be more influential in the understanding of human personality.[2] However, the most cited and influential figures in publishing the first biology-based personality theories are Hans Eysenck and Jeffrey Alan Gray. Eysenck used both behavioral and psychophysiological methodologies to test and develop his theories.[3] He published a book in 1947 called Dimensions of Personality, describing the personality dimensions of Extraversion and Neuroticism. Gray, a student of Eysenck, studied personality traits as individual differences in sensitivity to rewarding and punishing stimuli.[3] The significance of Gray's work and theories was his use of biology to define behavior, which stimulated a lot of subsequent research.[4] In 1951, Hans Eysenck and Donald Prell published an experiment in which identical (monozygotic) and fraternal (dizygotic) twins, ages 11 and 12, were tested for neuroticism. It is described in detail in an article published in the Journal of Mental Science. in which Eysenck and Prell concluded that, "The factor of neuroticism is not a statistical artifact, but constitutes a biological unit which is inherited as a whole....neurotic predisposition is to a large extent hereditarily determined."[5] The idea of biology-based personality research is relatively new, but growing in interest and number of publications.[6] In August 2004, there was a conference specifically on the topic, called The Biological Basis of Personality and Individual Differences.[7] This allowed for presenting and sharing of ideas between psychologists, psychiatrists, molecular geneticists, and neuroscientists, and eventually gave birth to the book under the same title.[7] The book is a collection of current research (as of 2006) in the field contributed by many authors and edited by Turhan Canli. Recently, psychology professor Colin G. DeYoung has even named the idea as the field of 'Personality Neuroscience'.{{citation needed|date=December 2012}} Experimental techniquesThere are many experimental techniques for measuring the biology of the brain, but there are five main methods used to investigate the biological basis of personality.[8] The biological data from these methods are commonly correlated with personality traits. These personality traits are often determined by personality questionnaires. However, personality questionnaires may be biased because they are self-reported. As a result, scientists emphasize using several different measures of personality,[8][9] rather than solely self-reported measures of personality. For example, another measure of personality traits is observation of behavior. Both humans and animals have been observed to measure personality traits, but animals are particularly useful for studying the long-term behavioral-biological relationship of personality.[10] Another interesting method that has become more sophisticated and affordable to researchers is the method of whole genome expression analysis. This method involves collecting data for a large number of genes simultaneously which provides many advantages in studying personality. In an article written by Alison M. Bell and Nadia Aubin-Horth, they describe the advantages very clearly by stating, "For one, it is probable that the genetic basis of personality is polygenic, so it makes sense to simultaneously study many genes. In addition, gene products rarely act alone. Instead, they perform their function by interacting together in pathways and networks. As a result, the molecular changes that characterize a phenotype are frequently not based on a single marker or gene, but rather on an entire pathway. Whole genome expression profiling therefore has the potential to reveal new candidates genes and pathways."[11]
Genetic and molecular correlations to personalityNeurotransmittersThe biology-based personality theories (discussed below) are based on correlating personality traits with behavioral systems related to motivation, reward, and punishment. On a broad level, this involves the autonomic nervous system, fear-processing circuits in the amygdala, the reward pathway from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. All of these circuits heavily rely on neurotransmitters and their precursors, but there has been the most research support for dopamine and serotonin pathways:
GenesPrevious studies show that genes account for at most 50 percent of a given trait.[1] However, it is widely accepted that variance in gene sequence affect behavior, and genes are a significant risk factor for personality disorders.[13] With the growing interest in using molecular genetics in tracing the biological basis of personality,[7] there may be more gene-trait links found in the future. Varying polymorphisms and sequence repeats in the gene for dopamine receptor D4 and serotonin transporter gene 5-HTTLPR, have both been found to influence the extraversion trait in adults. Specifically, study participants with at least one copy of the 7-repeat variant of the dopamine receptor D4 gene had higher scores of self-reported extraversion.[7] This suggests that dopamine and serotonin interact to regulate the conflicting behavioral traits of careless exploration vs. cautious inhibition.[12] Synaptic plasticitySynaptic plasticity refers to the ability of neurons to strengthen or weaken the connections between them. According to Hebbian theory, these connections are strengthened and maintained through repeated stimulation between neurons. Specifically, there is an emphasis on long-term potentiation (LTP), which is the prolonged strengthening of synaptic connections that facilitate learning from experience. On a larger scale, there are many pathways and brain regions that are interdependent and contribute to a cohesive, stable personality. For example, the amygdala and hippocampus of the limbic system mediate emotional intensity and consolidate memory of these experiences. But the basic mechanism by which these pathways and brain regions perform these functions, is synaptic plasticity. Ultimately, it boils down to this feature of neurons that allows the brain to learn from repeated experiences, retain memories, and ultimately maintain personality.[14] Joseph LeDoux, an award-winning neuroscientist, asserts that although humans share the same brain systems, it is the unique wiring of neurons that is different in each person and makes their personality.[14] Personality theories with biological basisThere are many theories of personality that on the identification of a set of traits that encompass human personality. Few however, are biologically based. This section will describe some theories of personality that have a biological basis. Additionally, it will present biological support for a popular non-biologically based personality theory, the Five Factor Model. Eysenck's three-factor model of personalityEysenck's three-factor model of personality was a causal theory of personality based on activation of reticular formation and limbic system. The reticular formation is a region in the brainstem that is involved in mediating arousal and consciousness. The limbic system is involved in mediating emotion, behavior, motivation, and long-term memory.
Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theoryGray's reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) is based on the idea that there are three brain systems that all differently respond to rewarding and punishing stimuli.[3]
Cloninger model of personality{{anchor|Cloninger Model of Personality}}This model of personality is based on the idea that different responses to punishing, rewarding, and novel stimuli the main characteristics of the human mind is caused by an interaction of the three dimensions below:
In one MRI study,[15] Novelty Seeking correlated with increased grey matter volume in regions of the cingulate cortex, Harm Avoidance correlated with decreased grey matter volume in the orbitofrontal, occipital, and parietal cortex. Reward Dependence correlated with decreased grey matter volume in the caudate nucleus. Five factor model of personalityThe five factor model is a widely used personality assessment that describes five core traits that a person possesses:
Using an MRI, one study[16] found correlation between the volumes of certain brain areas with each of the five traits in the Five Factor Model. Their results found that Openness/Intellect did not have any significant correlation with the volume of any brain structures. Conscientiousness was associated with increased volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex, a region involved in planning and the voluntary control of behavior. Extraversion was associated with increased volume of medial orbitofrontal cortex, a region involved in processing reward information. Agreeableness was associated with increased volume in regions that process information about the intentions and mental states of other individuals. Neuroticism was associated with increased volume of brain regions associated with threat, punishment, and negative emotions. See also
References1. ^1 {{cite journal|last=LeDoux|first=J.|year=2003|title=The Self|journal=Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences|volume=1001|issue=1|pages=295–304| pmid=14625368|doi=10.1196/annals.1279.017}} 2. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Davidson | first1 = R. J. | year = 2001 | title = Toward a biology of personality and emotion | url = http://www.cogsci.bme.hu/DoCS/oktatas/kurzusok/olvasoszeminarium/2003/towardabiology.pdf | journal = Ann N Y Acad Sci | volume = 935 | issue = | pages = 191–207 | doi = 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03481.x | pmid = 11411166 }}{{dead link|date=July 2017 |bot=InternetArchiveBot |fix-attempted=yes }} 3. ^1 2 3 {{cite journal|last1=Corr|first1=Philip J.|last2=Perkins|first2=Adam M.|title=The role of theory in the psychophysiology of personality: From Ivan Pavlov to Jeffrey Gray|journal=International Journal of Psychophysiology|volume=62|issue=3|year=2006|pages=367–376|issn=0167-8760|doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.005|pmid=16515814}} 4. ^{{cite book |last=Fowles |first=Don |editor-first=Turhan |editor-last=Canli |title= Biology of personality and individual differences|publisher=Guilford Press |year=2006 |chapter=Chapter 2: Jeffrey Gray's Contributions to Theories of Anxiety, Personality, and Psychopathology|isbn=978-1593852528}}. 5. ^The Journal of Mental Health, July 1951, Vol. XCVII, "The Inheritance of Neuroticism: An Experimental Study", H. J. Eysenck and D. B. Prell, p. 402. 6. ^1 {{cite book |last=Canli |first=Turhan |editor-first=Turhan |editor-last=Canli |title= Biology of personality and individual differences|publisher=Guilford Press |year=2006 |chapter=Chapter 5: Genomic Imaging of Extraversion|isbn=978-1593852528}}. 7. ^1 2 3 {{cite book |last=Canli |first=Turhan |editor-first=Turhan |editor-last=Canli |title= Biology of personality and individual differences|publisher=Guilford Press |year=2006 |chapter=Chapter 1: Introduction|isbn=978-1593852528}}. 8. ^1 2 3 4 5 6 {{cite journal|last1=DeYoung|first1=Colin G.|title=Personality Neuroscience and the Biology of Traits|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass|volume=4|issue=12|year=2010|pages=1165–1180|issn=1751-9004|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x}} 9. ^1 {{cite book |last=Zuckerman |first=Marvin |editor-first=Turhan |editor-last=Canli |title= Biology of personality and individual differences|publisher=Guilford Press |year=2006 |chapter=Chapter 3: Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking|isbn=978-1593852528}}. 10. ^{{cite book |last=Mehta |first=Pranjal |last2=Gosling |first2=Samuel |editor-first=Turhan |editor-last=Canli |title= Biology of personality and individual differences|publisher=Guilford Press |year=2006 |chapter=Chapter 20: How Can Animal Studies Contribute to Research on the Biological Bases of Personality|isbn=978-1593852528}}. 11. ^Bell, A.M., & Aubin-Horth, N. (2010). What can whole genome expression data tell usabout the ecology and evolution of personality?. Philosophical Transactions ofthe Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. Retrieved September 10, 2014, from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/365/1560/4001.full.pdf+html. 12. ^1 2 {{cite journal | last1 = Ebstein | first1 = Richard P. | last2 = Auerbach | first2 = Judith G. | year = 2002 | title = Dopamine D4 receptor and serotonin transporter promoter polymorphisms and temperament in early childhood | url = | journal = Molecular Genetics and the Human Personality | volume = | issue = | pages = 137–149 }} 13. ^Whittle, S., Allen, N. B., Lubman, D. I., & Yucel, M. (2006). Neurobiological basis of temperament: Towards a better understanding of psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 511-525. 14. ^1 LeDoux, J. E. (2003). Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are: Penguin Books. 15. ^{{cite journal|last1=Gardini|first1=Simona|last2=Cloninger|first2=C. Robert|last3=Venneri|first3=Annalena|title=Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions|journal=Brain Research Bulletin|volume=79|issue=5|year=2009|pages=265–270|issn=0361-9230|doi=10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.03.005|pmid=19480986}} 16. ^{{cite journal | last1 = DeYoung | first1 = C. G. | last2 = Hirsh | first2 = J. B. | last3 = Shane | first3 = M. S. | last4 = Papademetris | first4 = X. | last5 = Rajeevan | first5 = N. | last6 = Gray | first6 = J. R. | year = 2010 | title = Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five | url = http://pss.sagepub.com/content/21/6/820.short | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 21 | issue = 6| pages = 820–828 | doi=10.1177/0956797610370159 | pmid=20435951 | pmc=3049165}} 4 : Psychology|Neuroscience|Hereditarianism|Psychological theories |
|||||||||||||||||
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。