请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Replication crisis
释义

  1. Scope of the crisis

     Overall   In psychology   Psychology replication rates   A disciplinary social dilemma    "Methodological terrorism" controversy    In medicine    In marketing    In economics    In sports science    In hydrology    Political repercussions  

  2. Causes of the crisis

  3. Addressing the replication crisis

      Tackling publication bias with pre-registration of studies    Emphasizing replication attempts in teaching    Reducing the p-value required for claiming significance of new results    Addressing the misinterpretation of p-values    Encouraging larger sample sizes    Sharing raw data in online repositories    Funding for replication studies    Emphasize triangulation, not just replication  

  4. See also

  5. References

  6. Further reading

The replication crisis (or replicability crisis or reproducibility crisis) is an ongoing (2019) methodological crisis primarily affecting parts of the social and life sciences in which scholars have found that the results of many scientific studies are difficult or impossible to replicate or reproduce on subsequent investigation, either by independent researchers or by the original researchers themselves.[1][2] The crisis has long-standing roots; the phrase was coined in the early 2010s[3] as part of a growing awareness of the problem.

Because the reproducibility of experiments is an essential part of the scientific method,[4] the inability to replicate the studies of others has potentially grave consequences for many fields of science in which significant theories are grounded on unreproducible experimental work.

The replication crisis has been particularly widely discussed in the field of psychology (and in particular, social psychology) and in medicine, where a number of efforts have been made to re-investigate classic results, and to attempt to determine both the reliability of the results, and, if found to be unreliable, the reasons for the failure of replication.[5][6]

Scope of the crisis

Overall

According to a 2016 poll of 1,500 scientists reported in the journal Nature, 70% of them had failed to reproduce at least one other scientist's experiment (50% had failed to reproduce one of their own experiments).

In 2009, 2% of scientists admitted to falsifying studies at least once and 14% admitted to personally knowing someone who did. Misconducts were reported more frequently by medical researchers than others.[7]

In psychology

Several factors have combined to put psychology at the center of the controversy.[8] Much of the focus has been on the area of social psychology,[9] although other areas of psychology such as clinical psychology,[10][11] developmental psychology,[12] and educational research have also been implicated.[13][14] According to a 2018 survey of 200 meta-analyses, "psychological research is, on average, afflicted with low statistical power."[15]

Firstly, questionable research practices (QRPs) have been identified as common in the field.[16] Such practices, while not intentionally fraudulent, involve capitalizing on the gray area of acceptable scientific practices or exploiting flexibility in data collection, analysis, and reporting, often in an effort to obtain a desired outcome. Examples of QRPs include selective reporting or partial publication of data (reporting only some of the study conditions or collected dependent measures in a publication), optional stopping (choosing when to stop data collection, often based on statistical significance of tests), p-value rounding (rounding p-values down to 0.05 to suggest statistical significance), file drawer effect (nonpublication of data), post-hoc storytelling (framing exploratory analyses as confirmatory analyses), and manipulation of outliers (either removing outliers or leaving outliers in a dataset to cause a statistical test to be significant).[16][17][18][19] A survey of over 2,000 psychologists indicated that a majority of respondents admitted to using at least one QRP.[16] False positive conclusions, often resulting from the pressure to publish or the author's own confirmation bias, are an inherent hazard in the field, requiring a certain degree of skepticism on the part of readers.[20]

Secondly, psychology and social psychology in particular, has found itself at the center of several scandals involving outright fraudulent research, most notably the admitted data fabrication by Diederik Stapel[21] as well as allegations against others. However, most scholars acknowledge that fraud is, perhaps, the lesser contribution to replication crises.

Thirdly, several effects in psychological science have been found to be difficult to replicate even before the current replication crisis. For example the scientific journal Judgment and Decision Making has published several studies over the years that fail to provide support for the unconscious thought theory. Replications appear particularly difficult when research trials are pre-registered and conducted by research groups not highly invested in the theory under questioning.

These three elements together have resulted in renewed attention for replication supported by psychologist Daniel Kahneman.[22] Scrutiny of many effects have shown that several core beliefs are hard to replicate. A recent special edition of the journal Social Psychology focused on replication studies and a number of previously held beliefs were found to be difficult to replicate.[23] A 2012 special edition of the journal Perspectives on Psychological Science also focused on issues ranging from publication bias to null-aversion that contribute to the replication crises in psychology.[24] In 2015, the first open empirical study of reproducibility in Psychology was published, called the Reproducibility Project. Researchers from around the world collaborated to replicate 100 empirical studies from three top Psychology journals. Fewer than half of the attempted replications were successful at producing statistically significant results in the expected directions, though most of the attempted replications did produce trends in the expected directions.[25]

Many research trials and meta-analyses are compromised by poor quality and conflicts of interest that involve both authors and professional advocacy organizations, resulting in many false positives regarding the effectiveness of certain types of psychotherapy.[26]

Although the British newspaper The Independent wrote that the results of the reproducibility project show that much of the published research is just "psycho-babble",[27] the replication crisis does not necessarily mean that psychology is unscientific.[28][29][30] Rather this process is a healthy if sometimes acrimonious part of the scientific process in which old ideas or those that cannot withstand careful scrutiny are pruned,[31][32] although this pruning process is not always effective.[33][34] The consequence is that some areas of psychology once considered solid, such as social priming, have come under increased scrutiny due to failed replications.[35]

Nobel laureate and professor emeritus in psychology Daniel Kahneman argued that the original authors should be involved in the replication effort because the published methods are often too vague.[36] Others such as Dr. Andrew Wilson disagree and argue that the methods should be written down in detail[37]. An investigation of replication rates in psychology in 2012 indicated higher success rates of replication in replication studies when there was author overlap with the original authors of a study[38] (91.7% successful replication rates in studies with author overlap compared to 64.6% success replication rates without author overlap).

Psychology replication rates

A report by the Open Science Collaboration in August 2015 that was coordinated by Brian Nosek estimated the reproducibility of 100 studies in psychological science from three high-ranking psychology journals.[39] Overall, 36% of the replications yielded significant findings (p value below 0.05) compared to 97% of the original studies that had significant effects. The mean effect size in the replications was approximately half the magnitude of the effects reported in the original studies.

The same paper examined the reproducibility rates and effect sizes by journal (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology [JPSP], Learning, Memory, and Cognition [JEP:LMC], Psychological Science [PSCI]) and discipline (social psychology, cognitive psychology). Study replication rates were 23% for JPSP, 38% for JEP:LMC, and 38% for PSCI. Studies in the field of cognitive psychology had a higher replication rate (50%) than studies in the field of social psychology (25%).

An analysis of the publication history in the top 100 psychology journals between 1900 and 2012 indicated that approximately 1.6% of all psychology publications were replication attempts.[38] Articles were considered a replication attempt if the term "replication" appeared in the text. A subset of those studies (500 studies) was randomly selected for further examination and yielded a lower replication rate of 1.07% (342 of the 500 studies [68.4%] were actually replications). In the subset of 500 studies, analysis indicated that 78.9% of published replication attempts were successful. The rate of successful replication was significantly higher when at least one author of the original study was part of the replication attempt (91.7% relative to 64.6%).

A study published in 2018 in Nature Human Behaviour sought to replicate 21 social and behavioral science papers from Nature and Science, finding that only 13 could be successfully replicated.[40][41]. Similarly, in a study conducted under the auspices of the Center for Open Science, a team of 186 researchers from 60 different laboratories (representing 36 different nationalities from 6 different continents) conducted replications of 28 classic and contemporary findings in psychology.[42] The focus of the study was not only on whether or not the findings from the original papers replicated, but also on the extent to which findings varied as a function of variations in samples and contexts. Overall, 14 of the 28 findings failed to replicate despite massive sample sizes. However, if a finding replicated, it replicated in most samples, while if a finding was not replicated, it failed to replicate with little variation across samples and contexts. This evidence is inconsistent with a popular explanation that failures to replicate in psychology are likely due to changes in the sample between the original and replication study.[43]

A disciplinary social dilemma

Highlighting the social structure that discourages replication in psychology, Brian D. Earp and Jim A. C. Everett enumerated five points as to why replication attempts are uncommon[44][45]

  1. "Independent, direct replications of others’ findings can be time-consuming for the replicating researcher
  2. "[Replications] are likely to take energy and resources directly away from other projects that reflect one’s own original thinking
  3. "[Replications] are generally harder to publish (in large part because they are viewed as being unoriginal)
  4. "Even if [replications] are published, they are likely to be seen as 'bricklaying' exercises, rather than as major contributions to the field
  5. "[Replications] bring less recognition and reward, and even basic career security, to their authors"&91;46&93; For these reasons the authors advocated that psychology is facing a disciplinary social dilemma, where the interests of the discipline are at odds with the interests of the individual researcher.

"Methodological terrorism" controversy

With the replication crisis of psychology earning attention, Princeton University psychologist Susan Fiske drew controversy for calling out critics of psychology.[47][48][49][50] She called these unnamed "adversaries" names such as "methodological terrorist" and "self-appointed data police", and said that criticism of psychology should only be expressed in private or through contacting the journals.[47] Columbia University statistician and political scientist Andrew Gelman, "well-respected among the researchers driving the replication debate", responded to Fiske, saying that she had found herself willing to tolerate the "dead paradigm" of faulty statistics and had refused to retract publications even when errors were pointed out.[47][51] He added that her tenure as editor has been abysmal and that a number of published papers edited by her were found to be based on extremely weak statistics; one of Fiske's own published papers had a major statistical error and "impossible" conclusions.[47]

In medicine

Out of 49 medical studies from 1990–2003, with more than 1000 citations, 45 claimed that studied therapy was effective. Out of these studies, 16% were contradicted by subsequent studies, 16% had found stronger effects than did subsequent studies, 44% were replicated, and 24% remained largely unchallenged.[52] Food and Drug Administration in 1977–90 found flaws in 10–20% of medical studies.[53] In a paper published in 2012, Glenn Begley, a biotech consultant working at Amgen, and Lee Ellis, at the University of Texas, argued that only 11% of the pre-clinical cancer studies could be replicated.[54][55]

A 2016 article by John Ioannidis, Professor of Medicine and of Health Research and Policy at Stanford University School of Medicine and a Professor of Statistics at Stanford University School of Humanities and Sciences, elaborated on "Why Most Clinical Research Is Not Useful."[56] In the article Ioannidis laid out some of the problems and called for reform, characterizing certain points for medical research to be useful again – one example he made was the need for medicine to be "Patient Centered" (e.g. in the form of the Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute) instead of the current practice to mainly take care of "the needs of physicians, investigators, or sponsors." Ioannidis is known for his research focus on science itself since the 2005 paper "Why Most Published Research Findings Are False."[57]

In marketing

Marketing is another discipline with a "desperate need" for replication.[58] Many famous marketing studies fail to be repeated upon replication, a notable example being the "too-many-choices" effect, in which a high number of choices of product makes a consumer less likely to purchase.[59] In addition to the previously mentioned arguments, replications studies in marketing are needed to examine the applicability of theories and models across countries and cultures, which is especially important because of possible influences of globalization.[60]

In economics

A 2016 study in the journal Science found that two-thirds of 18 experimental studies from two top-tier economics journals (American Economic Review and the Quarterly Journal of Economics) successfully replicated.[61][62] A 2017 study in the Economic Journal suggested that "the majority of the average effects in the empirical economics literature are exaggerated by a factor of at least 2 and at least one-third are exaggerated by a factor of 4 or more."[63]

In sports science

A 2018 study took the field of exercise and sports science to task for insufficient replication studies, limited reporting of null results and trivial results, and insufficient research transparency.[64] Statisticians have criticized sports science for common use of a controversial statistical method called "magnitude-based inference" that has allowed sports scientists to extract apparently significant results from noisy data where ordinary hypothesis testing would have found none.[65]

In hydrology

A 2019 study in Scientific Data found that a small number of articles in hydrology and water resources journals could be reproduced due to data unavailability. The study "estimated, with 95% confidence, that results might be reproduced for only 0.6% to 6.8% of all 1,989 articles."[66]

Political repercussions

In the US, science’s reproducibility crisis has become a topic of political contention, linked to the attempt to diminish regulations – e.g. of emissions of pollutants, with the argument that these regulations are based on non-reproducible science.[67][68] Previous attempts with the same aim accused science of being non-transparent [69].

Causes of the crisis

In a work published in 2015 Glenn Begley and John Ioannidis offer five bullets as to summarize the present predicaments:[70]

  • Generation of new data/publications at an unprecedented rate.
  • Compelling evidence that the majority of these discoveries will not stand the test of time.
  • Causes: failure to adhere to good scientific practice & the desperation to publish or perish.
  • This is a multifaceted, multistakeholder problem.
  • No single party is solely responsible, and no single solution will suffice.

In fact some predictions of a possible crisis in the quality control mechanism of science can be traced back several decades, especially among scholars in science and technology studies (STS). Derek de Solla Price – considered the father of scientometrics – predicted that science could reach 'senility' as a result of its own exponential growth.[71] Some present day literature seems to vindicate this 'overflow' prophesy, lamenting at decay in both attention and quality.[72][73]

Philosopher and historian of science Jerome R. Ravetz predicted in his 1971 book Scientific Knowledge and Its Social Problems that science – in moving from the little science made of restricted communities of scientists to big science or techno-science – would suffer major problems in its internal system of quality control. Ravetz anticipated that modern science's system of rewarding scientists for research might become dysfunctional, the present 'publish or perish' challenge, creating perverse incentives to publish any findings however dubious. For Ravetz quality in science is maintained when there is a community of scholars linked by norms and standards, and a willingness to stand by these.

Historian Philip Mirowski offered more recently a similar diagnosis in his 2011 book Science Mart (2011).[74] 'Mart' is here a reference to the retail giant 'Walmart' and an allusion to the commodification of science. In the analysis of Mirowski, when science becomes a commodity being traded in a market, its quality collapses. Mirowski argues his case by tracing the decay of science to the decision of major corporations to close their in-house laboratories in order to outsource their work to universities, and subsequently to move their research away from universities to even cheaper contract research organizations (CRO).

The crisis of science's quality control system is affecting the use of science for policy. This is the thesis of a recent work by a group of STS scholars, who identify in 'evidence based (or informed) policy' a point of present tension.[75][76][77][68] Economist Noah Smith suggests that a factor in the crisis has been the overvaluing of research in academia and undervaluing of teaching ability, especially in fields with few major recent discoveries.[78]

Addressing the replication crisis

Replication has been referred to as "the cornerstone of science."[79][80] Replication studies attempt to evaluate whether published results reflect true findings or false positives. The integrity of scientific findings and reproducibility of research are important as they form the knowledge foundation on which future studies are built.

Tackling publication bias with pre-registration of studies

{{main|Registered report}}

A recent innovation in scientific publishing to address the replication crisis is through the use of registered reports.[81][82] The registered report format requires authors to submit a description of the study methods and analyses prior to data collection. Once the method and analysis plan is vetted through peer-review, publication of the findings is provisionally guaranteed, based on whether the authors follow the proposed protocol. One goal of registered reports is to circumvent the publication bias toward significant findings that can lead to implementation of questionable research practices and to encourage publication of studies with rigorous methods.

The journal Psychological Science has encouraged the preregistration of studies and the reporting of effect sizes and confidence intervals.[83] The editor in chief also noted that the editorial staff will be asking for replication of studies with surprising findings from examinations using small sample sizes before allowing the manuscripts to be published.

Moreover, only a very small proportion of academic journals in psychology and neurosciences explicitly stated that they welcome submissions of replication studies in their aim and scope or instructions to authors.[84][85] This phenomenon does not encourage the reporting or even attempt on replication studies.

Emphasizing replication attempts in teaching

Based on coursework in experimental methods at MIT and Stanford, it has been suggested that methods courses in psychology emphasize replication attempts rather than original studies.[86][87] Such an approach would help students learn scientific methodology and provide numerous independent replications of meaningful scientific findings that would test the replicability of scientific findings. Some have recommended that graduate students should be required to publish a high-quality replication attempt on a topic related to their doctoral research prior to graduation.[45]

Reducing the p-value required for claiming significance of new results

Many publications require a p-value of p < 0.05 to claim statistical significance. The paper "Redefine statistical significance",[88] signed by a large number of scientists and mathematicians, proposes that in "fields where the threshold for defining statistical significance for new discoveries is P < 0.05, we propose a change to P < 0.005. This simple step would immediately improve the reproducibility of scientific research in many fields."

Their rationale is that "a leading cause of non-reproducibility (is that the) statistical standards of evidence for claiming new discoveries in many fields of science are simply too low. Associating 'statistically significant' findings with P < 0.05 results in a high rate of false positives even in the absence of other experimental, procedural and reporting problems."

Addressing the misinterpretation of p-values

Although statisticians are unanimous that use of the p < 0.05 provides weaker evidence than is generally appreciated, there is a lack of unanimity about what should be done about it. Some have advocated that Bayesian methods should replace p-values. This has not happened on a wide scale, partly because it is complicated, and partly because many users distrust the specification of prior distributions in the absence of hard data. A simplified version of the Bayesian argument, based on testing a point null hypothesis was suggested by Colquhoun (2014, 2017).[89][90] The logical problems of inductive inference were discussed in "The problem with p-values" (2016).[91]

The hazards of reliance on p-values were emphasized by pointing out that even observation of p = 0.001 was not necessarily strong evidence against the null hypothesis.[90] Despite the fact that the likelihood ratio in favour of the alternative hypothesis over the null is close to 100, if the hypothesis was implausible, with a prior probability of a real effect being 0.1, even the observation of p = 0.001 would have a false positive risk of 8 percent. It wouldn't even reach the 5 percent level.

It was recommended[90] that the terms "significant" and "non-significant" should not be used. p-values and confidence intervals should still be specified, but they should be accompanied by an indication of the false positive risk. It was suggested that the best way to do this is to calculate the prior probability that would be necessary to believe in order to achieve a false positive risk of, say, 5%. The calculations can be done with R scripts that are provided,[90] or, more simply, with a web calculator.[92] This so-called reverse Bayesian approach, which was suggested by Matthews (2001),[93] is one way to avoid the problem that the prior probability is rarely known.

Encouraging larger sample sizes

To improve the quality of replications, larger sample sizes than those used in the original study are often needed.[94] Larger sample sizes are needed because estimates of effect sizes in published work are often exaggerated due to publication bias and large sampling variability associated with small sample sizes in an original study.[95][96][96][97] Further, using significance thresholds usually leads to inflated effects, because particularly with small sample sizes, only the largest effects will become significant.[98]

Sharing raw data in online repositories

Online repositories where data, protocols, and findings can be stored and evaluated by the public seek to improve the integrity and reproducibility of research. Examples of such repositories include the Open Science Framework, Registry of Research Data Repositories, and Psychfiledrawer.org. Sites like Open Science Framework offer badges for using open science practices in an effort to incentivize scientists. However, there has been concern that those who are most likely to provide their data and code for analyses are the researchers that are likely the most sophisticated.[99] John Ioannidis at Stanford University suggested that "the paradox may arise that the most meticulous and sophisticated and method-savvy and careful researchers may become more susceptible to criticism and reputation attacks by reanalyzers who hunt for errors, no matter how negligible these errors are."[99]

Funding for replication studies

In July 2016 the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research made 3 million Euros available for replication studies. The funding is for replication based on reanalysis of existing data and replication by collecting and analysing new data. Funding is available in the areas of social sciences, health research and healthcare innovation.[100]

In 2013 the Laura and John Arnold Foundation funded the launch of The Center for Open Science with a $5.25 million grant and by 2017 had provided an additional $10 million in funding.[101] It also funded the launch of the Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford at Stanford University run by John Ioannidis and Steven Goodman to study ways to improve scientific research.[101] It also provided funding for the AllTrials initiative led in part by Ben Goldacre.[101]

Emphasize triangulation, not just replication

Marcus R. Munafò and George Davey Smith argue, in a piece published by Nature, that research should emphasize triangulation, not just replication. They claim that,

"replication alone will get us only so far (and) might actually make matters worse... We believe that an essential protection against flawed ideas is triangulation. This is the strategic use of multiple approaches to address one question. Each approach has its own unrelated assumptions, strengths and weaknesses. Results that agree across different methodologies are less likely to be artefacts.... Maybe one reason replication has captured so much interest is the often-repeated idea that falsification is at the heart of the scientific enterprise. This idea was popularized by Karl Popper's 1950s maxim that theories can never be proved, only falsified. Yet an overemphasis on repeating experiments could provide an unfounded sense of certainty about findings that rely on a single approach.... philosophers of science have moved on since Popper. Better descriptions of how scientists actually work include what epistemologist Peter Lipton called in 1991 "inference to the best explanation.""[102]

See also

  • Data dredging
  • Invalid science
  • Meta-research
  • Publication bias
  • Reproducibility Project
  • Science wars

References

1. ^{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1038/515009a| title = Metascience could rescue the 'replication crisis'| journal = Nature| volume = 515| issue = 7525| pages = 9| year = 2014| last1 = Schooler | first1 = J. W.| pmid=25373639| bibcode = 2014Natur.515....9S}}
2. ^{{cite web|last1=Smith|first1=Noah|title=Why 'Statistical Significance' Is Often Insignificant|url=https://www.bloomberg.com/view/articles/2017-11-02/why-statistical-significance-is-often-insignificant|website=Bloomberg|accessdate=7 November 2017}}
3. ^{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1177/1745691612465253| title = Editors' Introduction to the Special Section on Replicability in Psychological Science: A Crisis of Confidence?| journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science| volume = 7| issue = 6| pages = 528–530| year = 2012| last1=Pashler|first1=Harold |last2=Wagenmakers | first2=Eric Jan| pmid=26168108}}
4. ^Staddon, John (2017) Scientific Method: How science works, fails to work or pretends to work. Taylor and Francis.
5. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.newyorker.com/tech/elements/the-crisis-in-social-psychology-that-isnt|title=The Crisis in Social Psychology That Isn't|author=Gary Marcus|publisher=The New Yorker|date=May 1, 2013}}
6. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2010/12/13/the-truth-wears-off|title=The Truth Wears Off|author=Jonah Lehrer|publisher=The New Yorker|date=December 13, 2010}}
7. ^{{cite journal|title=How Many Scientists Fabricate and Falsify Research? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Survey Data|first=Daniele|last=Fanelli|date=29 May 2009|publisher=|journal=PLOS One|volume=4|issue=5|pages=e5738|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0005738|pmid=19478950|pmc=2685008|bibcode=2009PLoSO...4.5738F}}
8. ^{{cite web|last1=Achenbach|first1=Joel|title=No, science's reproducibility problem is not limited to psychology|url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/speaking-of-science/wp/2015/08/28/no-sciences-reproducibility-problem-is-not-limited-to-psychology/|website=The Washington Post|accessdate=10 September 2015}}
9. ^{{Cite news|url=https://www.nytimes.com/2017/10/18/magazine/when-the-revolution-came-for-amy-cuddy.html|title=When the Revolution Came for Amy Cuddy|last=Dominus|first=Susan|date=2017-10-18|work=The New York Times|access-date=2017-10-19|language=en-US|issn=0362-4331}}
10. ^{{cite journal|last1=Leichsenring|first1=Falk|last2=Abbass|first2=Allan|author-link2=Allan Abbass|last3=Hilsenroth|first3=Mark J.|last4=Leweke|first4=Frank|last5=Luyten|first5=Patrick|last6=Keefe|first6=Jack R.|last7=Midgley|first7=Nick|last8=Rabung|first8=Sven|last9=Salzer|first9=Simone|last10=Steiner|first10=Christiane|title=Biases in research: risk factors for non-replicability in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy research|journal=Psychological Medicine|year=April 2017|volume=47|issue=6|pages=1000–1011|doi=10.1017/S003329171600324X|pmid=27955715}}
11. ^{{cite journal|last=Hengartner|first=Michael P.|title=Raising Awareness for the Replication Crisis in Clinical Psychology by Focusing on Inconsistencies in Psychotherapy Research: How Much Can We Rely on Published Findings from Efficacy Trials?|journal=Frontiers in Psychology|volume=9|pages=256|publisher=Frontiers Media|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00256|pmid=29541051|pmc=5835722|date=February 28, 2018}}
12. ^{{cite journal|last1=Frank|first1=Michael C.|author-link1=Michael C. Frank|last2=Bergelson|first2=Elika|last3=Bergmann|first3=Christina|last4=Cristia|first4=Alejandrina|last5=Floccia|first5=Caroline|last6=Gervain|first6=Judit|last7=Hamlin|first7=J. Kiley|last8=Hannon|first8=Erin E.|last9=Kline|first9=Melissa|last10=Levelt|first10=Claartje|last11=Lew-Williams|first11=Casey|last12=Nazzi|first12=Thierry|last13=Panneton|first13=Robin|last14=Rabagliati|first14=Hugh|last15=Soderstrom|first15=Melanie|last16=Sullivan|first16=Jessica|last17=Waxman|first17=Sandra|author-link17=Sandra Waxman|last18=Yurovsky|first18=Daniel|title=A Collaborative Approach to Infant Research: Promoting Reproducibility, Best Practices, and Theory‐Building|journal=Infancy|volume=22|issue=4|pages=421–435|date=9 March 2017|url=http://callab.uchicago.edu/papers/fetal-infancy-inpress.pdf|doi=10.1111/infa.12182|access-date=19 December 2018}}
13. ^{{cite journal|last=Tyson|first=Charlie|title=Failure to Replicate|journal=Inside Higher Ed|date=14 August 2014|url=https://www.insidehighered.com/news/2014/08/14/almost-no-education-research-replicated-new-article-shows|access-date=19 December 2018}}
14. ^{{cite journal|last1=Makel|first1=Matthew C.|last2=Plucker|first2=Jonathan A.|author-link2=Jonathan A. Plucker|title=Facts Are More Important Than Novelty: Replication in the Education Sciences|journal=Educational Researcher|date=1 August 2014|volume=43|issue=6|pages=304–316|doi=10.3102/0013189X14545513|access-date=19 December 2018|url=https://journals.sagepub.com/stoken/rbtfl/w5mrNxPVD8zSg/full}}
15. ^{{Cite journal|last=Stanley|first=T. D.|last2=Carter|first2=Evan C.|last3=Doucouliagos|first3=Hristos|date=2018|title=What meta-analyses reveal about the replicability of psychological research.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|language=en|volume=144|issue=12|pages=1325–1346|doi=10.1037/bul0000169|pmid=30321017|issn=1939-1455}}
16. ^{{Cite journal|title = Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices With Incentives for Truth Telling|url = https://www.hbs.edu/faculty/Publication%20Files/QRP12_a137cc09-45bc-4eae-9ee9-c0cb47f14add.pdf|journal = Psychological Science|date = 2012-05-01|issn = 0956-7976|pmid = 22508865|pages = 524–532|volume = 23|issue = 5|doi = 10.1177/0956797611430953|first = Leslie K.|last = John|first2 = George|last2 = Loewenstein|first3 = Drazen|last3 = Prelec}}
17. ^{{Cite journal|title = The Nine Circles of Scientific Hell|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/643|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168124|pages = 643–644|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612459519}}
18. ^{{Cite web|title = Research misconduct - The grey area of Questionable Research Practices|url = http://www.vib.be/en/news/Pages/Research-misconduct---The-grey-area-of-Questionable-Research-Practices.aspx|website = www.vib.be|accessdate = 2015-11-13}}
19. ^{{Cite journal|title = Questionable Research Practices Revisited|url = http://spp.sagepub.com/content/early/2015/10/19/1948550615612150|journal = Social Psychological and Personality Science|date = 2015-10-19|issn = 1948-5506|pages = 45–52|doi = 10.1177/1948550615612150|first = Klaus|last = Fiedler|first2 = Norbert|last2 = Schwarz|volume=7}}
20. ^{{cite journal|last1 = Simmons|first1 = Joseph|first2 = Leif|last2 = Nelson|first3 = Uri|last3 = Simonsohn|title = False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant|journal = Psychological Science|volume = 22|issue = 11|pages = 1359–1366|date = November 2011|issn = 0956-7976|doi = 10.1177/0956797611417632|pmid = 22006061}}
21. ^{{cite journal|url=http://chronicle.com/article/As-Dutch-Research-Scandal/129746/|title=Fraud Scandal Fuels Debate Over Practices of Social Psychology|journal=The Chronicle of Higher Education|first=Christopher|last=Shea|date=13 November 2011|publisher=|via=The Chronicle of Higher Education}}
22. ^{{cite web|title=A New Etiquette for Replication|url=https://www.scribd.com/doc/225285909/Kahneman-Commentary|first=Daniel|last=Kahneman|via=Scribd}}
23. ^{{Cite journal |year=2014 |url=http://www.psycontent.com/content/l67413865317/?p=3f8268e0307f4036906c158b1667c57a&pi=0 |journal=Social Psychology |publisher=Hogrefe Publishing |volume=45 |issue=3 |archive-url=https://archive.is/20140530230728/http://www.psycontent.com/content/l67413865317/?p=3f8268e0307f4036906c158b1667c57a&pi=0 |archive-date=30 May 2014}}
24. ^Table of Contents
25. ^{{Cite journal | last1 = Open Science Collaboration | title = Estimating the reproducibility of Psychological Science | doi = 10.1126/science.aac4716 | journal = Science | volume = 349 | issue = 6251 | pages = aac4716 | year = 2015 | pmid=26315443| url = http://eprints.keele.ac.uk/877/1/Open%20Science%20%28Science%20Pre-Print%29.pdf }}
26. ^{{cite web | url=http://blogs.plos.org/mindthebrain/2014/04/15/meta-analyses-conducted-professional-organizations-trustworthy/ | title=Are meta analyses conducted by professional organizations more trustworthy? | work=Mind the Brain | date=April 15, 2014 | accessdate=September 13, 2016 | first=James | last=Coyne}}
27. ^{{cite news| url=https://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/study-reveals-that-a-lot-of-psychology-research-really-is-just-psychobabble-10474646.html | location=London | work=The Independent | first=Steve | last=Connor | title=Study reveals that a lot of psychology research really is just 'psycho-babble' | date=27 August 2015}}
28. ^{{cite web|url=http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2014/07/replication_controversy_in_psychology_bullying_file_drawer_effect_blog_posts.single.html|title=Why Psychologists' Food Fight Matters|first1=Michelle N.|last1=Meyer|first2=Christopher|last2=Chabris|date=31 July 2014|publisher=|via=Slate}}
29. ^{{cite web|url=http://fivethirtyeight.com/datalab/psychology-is-starting-to-deal-with-its-replication-problem/|title=Psychology Is Starting To Deal With Its Replication Problem|date=27 August 2015|publisher=}}
30. ^{{cite web|url=http://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/|title=Science Isn't Broken|date=19 August 2015|publisher=}}
31. ^{{cite news|url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/may/28/psychology-replication-drive-methods-bullying|title=Psychology's replication drive: it's not about you|first=Pete|last=Etchells|date=28 May 2014|publisher=|newspaper=The Guardian}}
32. ^{{Cite journal|title = An Agenda for Purely Confirmatory Research|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/632|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168122|pages = 632–638|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612463078|first = Eric-Jan|last = Wagenmakers|first2 = Ruud|last2 = Wetzels|first3 = Denny|last3 = Borsboom|first4 = Han L. J. van der|last4 = Maas|first5 = Rogier A.|last5 = Kievit}}
33. ^{{Cite journal|title = Why Science Is Not Necessarily Self-Correcting|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/645|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168125|pages = 645–654|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612464056|first = John P. A.|last = Ioannidis}}
34. ^{{Cite journal|title = Is the Replicability Crisis Overblown? Three Arguments Examined|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/531|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168109|pages = 531–536|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612463401|first = Harold|last = Pashler|first2 = Christine R.|last2 = Harris}}
35. ^{{cite journal|url=http://chronicle.com/article/Power-of-Suggestion/136907/|title=Power of Suggestion|journal=The Chronicle of Higher Education|first=Tom|last=Bartlett|date=30 January 2013|publisher=|via=The Chronicle of Higher Education}}
36. ^{{cite news|url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/jun/10/physics-envy-do-hard-sciences-hold-the-solution-to-the-replication-crisis-in-psychology|title=Physics envy: Do 'hard' sciences hold the solution to the replication crisis in psychology?|first=Chris|last=Chambers|date=10 June 2014|publisher=|newspaper=The Guardian}}
37. ^{{cite news|url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/jun/10/physics-envy-do-hard-sciences-hold-the-solution-to-the-replication-crisis-in-psychology|title=Physics envy: Do 'hard' sciences hold the solution to the replication crisis in psychology?|first=Chris|last=Chambers|date=10 June 2014|publisher=|newspaper=The Guardian}}
38. ^{{Cite journal|title = Replications in Psychology Research How Often Do They Really Occur?|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/537|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168110|pages = 537–542|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612460688|first = Matthew C.|last = Makel|first2 = Jonathan A.|last2 = Plucker|first3 = Boyd|last3 = Hegarty}}
39. ^{{Cite journal|title = Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science|journal = Science|date = 2015-08-28|issn = 0036-8075|pmid = 26315443|pages = aac4716|volume = 349|issue = 6251|doi = 10.1126/science.aac4716|first = Open Science|last = Collaboration|url = http://eprints.keele.ac.uk/877/1/Open%20Science%20%28Science%20Pre-Print%29.pdf}}
40. ^{{Cite news|url=https://www.wired.com/story/social-science-reproducibility|title=The Science Behind Social Science Gets Shaken Up—Again|work=WIRED|access-date=2018-08-28|language=en-US}}
41. ^{{cite journal |last1=Camerer |first1=Colin F. |authorlink1=Colin Camerer|last2=Dreber |first2=Anna |last3=Holzmeister |first3=Felix |last4=Ho |first4=Teck-Hua |last5=Huber |first5=Jürgen |last6=Johannesson |first6=Magnus |last7=Kirchler |first7=Michael |last8=Nave |first8=Gideon |last9=Nosek |first9=Brian A. |last10=Pfeiffer |first10=Thomas |last11=Altmejd |first11=Adam |last12=Buttrick |first12=Nick |last13=Chan |first13=Taizan |last14=Chen |first14=Yiling |last15=Forsell |first15=Eskil |last16=Gampa |first16=Anup |last17=Heikensten |first17=Emma |last18=Hummer |first18=Lily |last19=Imai |first19=Taisuke |last20=Isaksson |first20=Siri |last21=Manfredi |first21=Dylan |last22=Rose |first22=Julia |last23=Wagenmakers |first23=Eric-Jan |last24=Wu |first24=Hang |display-authors=2 |title=Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015 |journal=Nature Human Behaviour |date=27 August 2018 |volume=2 |issue=9 |pages=637–644 |doi=10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z}}
42. ^{{cite journal |last1=Klein |first1=R.A. |title=Many Labs 2: Investigating Variation in Replicability Across Samples and Settings |journal=Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science |date=2018 |volume=1 |issue=4 |pages=443-490 |doi=10.1177/2515245918810225 |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/2515245918810225 |accessdate=23 March 2019}}
43. ^{{cite journal |last1=Witkowski |first1=Tomasz |title=Is the glass half empty or half full? Latest results in the replication crisis in Psychology |journal=Skeptical Inquirer |date=2019 |volume=43 |issue=2 |pages=5-6}}
44. ^see also Earp and Trafimow, 2015
45. ^{{Cite journal|title = A tragedy of the (academic) commons: interpreting the replication crisis in psychology as a social dilemma for early-career researchers|journal = Frontiers in Psychology|date = 2015-01-01|pmc = 4527093|pmid = 26300832|pages = 1152|doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01152|first = Jim Albert Charlton|last = Everett|first2 = Brian D.|last2 = Earp|volume=6}}
46. ^{{Cite web|title = Resolving the replication crisis in social psychology? A new proposal|url = http://www.spsp.org/blog/resolving-replication-crisis-social-psychology|publisher=Society for Personality and Social Psychology|accessdate = 2015-11-18}}
47. ^{{Cite news|url=http://uk.businessinsider.com/susan-fiske-methodological-terrorism-2016-9?r=US&IR=T|title=Scientists are furious after a famous psychologist accused her peers of 'methodological terrorism'|work=Business Insider|access-date=2017-10-04|language=en}}
48. ^{{Cite news|url=https://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/observer/obsonline/draft-of-observer-column-sparks-strong-social-media-response.html|title=Draft of Observer Column Sparks Strong Social Media Response|work=Association for Psychological Science|access-date=2017-10-04|language=en-US}}
49. ^{{Cite journal|last=Fiske|first=APS Past President Susan T.|date=2016-10-31|title=A Call to Change Science's Culture of Shaming|url=https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/a-call-to-change-sciences-culture-of-shaming|journal=APS Observer|language=en-US|volume=29|issue=9}}
50. ^{{Cite news|url=http://nymag.com/scienceofus/2016/10/inside-psychologys-methodological-terrorism-debate.html|title=Inside Psychology’s ‘Methodological Terrorism’ Debate|last=Singal|first=Jesse|work=Science of Us|access-date=2017-10-04|language=en}}
51. ^{{Cite news|url=http://andrewgelman.com/2017/10/04/breaking-pnas-changes-slogan/|title=BREAKING . . . . . . . PNAS updates its slogan! - Statistical Modeling, Causal Inference, and Social Science|date=2017-10-04|work=Statistical Modeling, Causal Inference, and Social Science|access-date=2017-10-04|language=en-US}}
52. ^{{cite journal|title=Contradicted and initially stronger effects in highly cited clinical research|last=Ioannidis JA|date=13 July 2005|publisher=|journal=JAMA|volume=294|issue=2|pages=218–228|doi=10.1001/jama.294.2.218|pmid=16014596}}
53. ^{{cite journal|title=Scientific data audit—A key management tool|first=J. Leslie|last=Glick|date=1 January 1992|publisher=|volume=2|issue=3|pages=153–168|doi=10.1080/08989629208573811|journal=Accountability in Research}}
54. ^Begley, C. G., and Ellis, L. M. 2012, Drug Development: Raise Standards for Preclinical Cancer Research, Nature, 483, 531–533.
55. ^Begley, C.G., 2013, Reproducibility: Six red flags for suspect work, Nature, 497, 433–434.
56. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Ioannidis | first1 = JPA | year = 2016 | title = Why Most Clinical Research Is Not Useful | journal = PLoS Med | volume = 13 | issue = 6| page = e1002049 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002049 | pmid = 27328301 | pmc = 4915619 }}
57. ^{{cite journal|title=Why Most Published Research Findings Are False|journal=PLoS Medicine|date=August 1, 2005|issn=1549-1277|pmc=1182327|pmid=16060722|volume=2|issue=8|doi=10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124|first=John P. A.|last=Ioannidis|pages=e124}}
58. ^{{Cite journal|title = The desperate need for replications|url = http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/content/28/1/149.article-info|journal = Journal of Consumer Research|date = 2001-06-01|pages = 149–158|volume = 28|issue = 1|doi = 10.1086/321953|first = John E.|last = Hunter}}
59. ^{{Cite journal|last=Armstrong|first=J.S.|last2=Green|first2=Kesten|date=30 January 2017|title=Guidelines for Science: Evidence and Checklists|url=http://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1258&context=marketing_papers|journal=Working Paper|volume=|pages=|via=}}
60. ^{{Cite journal|title = Effects of Subcultural Differences on Country and Product Evaluations: A Replication Study|journal = Journal of Global Marketing|date = 2016-03-22|pages = 115–127|volume = 29|issue = 3|doi = 10.1080/08911762.2015.1138012|first = Thomas|last = Aichner|first2 = Paolo|last2 = Coletti|first3 = Cipriano|last3 = Forza|first4 = Urban|last4 = Perkmann|first5 = Alessio|last5 = Trentin}}
61. ^{{Cite journal|last=Camerer|first=Colin F.|last2=Dreber|first2=Anna|last3=Forsell|first3=Eskil|last4=Ho|first4=Teck-Hua|last5=Huber|first5=Jürgen|last6=Johannesson|first6=Magnus|last7=Kirchler|first7=Michael|last8=Almenberg|first8=Johan|last9=Altmejd|first9=Adam|date=2016-03-25|title=Evaluating replicability of laboratory experiments in economics|journal=Science|language=en|volume=351|issue=6280|pages=1433–1436|doi=10.1126/science.aaf0918|issn=0036-8075|pmid=26940865|bibcode=2016Sci...351.1433C}}
62. ^{{Cite news|url=http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2016/03/about-40-economics-experiments-fail-replication-survey|title=About 40% of economics experiments fail replication survey|date=2016-03-03|work=Science {{!}} AAAS|access-date=2017-10-25|language=en}}
63. ^{{Cite journal|last=Ioannidis|first=John P. A.|last2=Stanley|first2=T. D.|last3=Doucouliagos|first3=Hristos|date=2017-10-01|title=The Power of Bias in Economics Research|journal=The Economic Journal|language=en|volume=127|issue=605|pages=F236–F265|doi=10.1111/ecoj.12461|issn=1468-0297}}
64. ^{{Cite journal|last=Halperin|first=Israel|last2=Vigotsky|first2=Andrew D.|last3=Foster|first3=Carl|last4=Pyne|first4=David B.|date=2018-02-01|title=Strengthening the Practice of Exercise and Sport-Science Research|journal=International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance|volume=13|issue=2|pages=127–134|doi=10.1123/ijspp.2017-0322|issn=1555-0273|pmid=28787228}}
65. ^{{Cite news|url=https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/how-shoddy-statistics-found-a-home-in-sports-research/|title=How Shoddy Statistics Found A Home In Sports Research|date=2018-05-16|work=FiveThirtyEight|access-date=2018-05-16|language=en-US}}
66. ^{{Cite journal|last=Stagge|first=James H.|last2=Rosenberg|first2=David E.|last3=Abdallah|first3=Adel M.|last4=Akbar|first4=Hadia|last5=Attallah|first5=Nour A.|last6=James|first6=Ryan|date=2019-02-26|title=Assessing data availability and research reproducibility in hydrology and water resources|journal=Scientific Data|language=en|volume=6|pages=190030|doi=10.1038/sdata.2019.30|pmid=30806638|issn=2052-4463}}
67. ^ Oreskes, N., 2018, Beware: Transparency rule is a trojan horse Nature, 557 (May 7706) (2018), p. 469.
68. ^{{cite journal |last1=Andrea |first1=Saltelli |title=Why science’s crisis should not become a political battling ground |journal=Futures |date=December 2018 |volume=104 |pages=85–90 |doi=10.1016/j.futures.2018.07.006}}
69. ^ D. Michaels, Doubt is their product: How industry’s assault on science threatens your health, Oxford University Press (2008).
70. ^{{cite journal|author1=Begley, C. G.|author2=Ioannidis, J. P.|lastauthoramp=y|year=2015|title=Reproducibility in Science: Improving the Standard for Basic and Preclinical Research|journal=Circulation Research|volume=116|issue=1|pages=116–126|doi=10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.114.303819|pmid=25552691}}
71. ^{{cite book|author1=De Solla Price|author2=Derek J.|year=1963|title=Little science big science|publisher=Columbia University Press}}
72. ^{{cite journal|author1=Siebert, S.|author2=Machesky, L. M.|author3=Insall, R. H.|lastauthoramp=y|year=2015|title=Overflow in science and its implications for trust|journal=eLife|volume=4|page=e10825|doi=10.7554/eLife.10825|pmid=26365552|pmc=4563216}}
73. ^{{cite journal|author1=Della Briotta Parolo, P.|author2=Kumar Pan|author3=R. Ghosh|author4=R. Huberman|author5=B.A. Kimmo Kaski|author6=Fortunato, S.|year=2015|title=Attention decay in science|journal=Journal of Informetrics|volume=9|issue=4|pages=734–745|doi=10.1016/j.joi.2015.07.006|arxiv=1503.01881}}
74. ^{{cite book|author=Mirowski, P.|year=2011|title=Science-Mart: Privatizing American Science|publisher=Harvard University Press}}
75. ^Saltelli, A. and Funtowicz, S. 2017. What is science’s crisis really about? FUTURES, Volume 91, Pages 5-11, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.futures.2017.05.010.
76. ^Benessia, A., Funtowicz, S., Giampietro, M., Guimarães Pereira, A., Ravetz, J., Saltelli, A., Strand, R., van der Sluijs, J., 2016. Science on the Verge. The Consortium for Science, Policy and Outcomes at Arizona State University.
77. ^{{cite magazine|author1=Andrea Saltelli|author2=Jerome R. Ravetz|author3=Silvio Funtowicz|lastauthoramp=y|url=https://www.newscientist.com/letter/mg23030791-600-7-a-new-community-for-science/|title=A new community for science|magazine=New Scientist|issue=3079|p=52|date=25 June 2016}}
78. ^{{cite web|last1=Smith|first1=Noah|title=Academic signaling and the post-truth world|url=http://noahpinionblog.blogspot.co.uk/2016/12/academic-signaling-and-post-truth-world.html|website=Noahpinion|publisher=Stony Brook University|accessdate=5 November 2017|date=2016-12-14}}
79. ^{{Cite journal|title = Most Published Research Findings Are False—But a Little Replication Goes a Long Way|journal = PLoS Med|date = 2007-02-27|pmc = 1808082|pmid = 17326704|pages = e28|volume = 4|issue = 2|doi = 10.1371/journal.pmed.0040028|first = Ramal|last = Moonesinghe|first2 = Muin J|last2 = Khoury|first3 = A. Cecile J. W|last3 = Janssens}}
80. ^{{Cite journal|title = The Value of Direct Replication|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/9/1/76|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2014-01-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26173243|pages = 76–80|volume = 9|issue = 1|doi = 10.1177/1745691613514755|first = Daniel J.|last = Simons}}
81. ^{{Cite web|title = Registered Replication Reports|publisher=Association for Psychological Science|url = http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/replication|accessdate = 2015-11-13}}
82. ^{{Cite news|title = Psychology's 'registration revolution'|url = https://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/may/20/psychology-registration-revolution|newspaper = the Guardian|accessdate = 2015-11-13|first = Chris|last = Chambers|date = 2014-05-20}}
83. ^{{Cite journal|title = Replication in Psychological Science|url = http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2015/11/09/0956797615616374|archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20151114060748/http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2015/11/09/0956797615616374|dead-url = yes|archive-date = 2015-11-14|journal = Psychological Science|date = 2015-11-09|issn = 0956-7976|pmid = 26553013|pages = 1827–32|doi = 10.1177/0956797615616374|first = D. Stephen|last = Lindsay|volume = 26|issue = 12}}
84. ^{{Cite journal|last=Yeung|first=Andy W. K.|date=2017|title=Do Neuroscience Journals Accept Replications? A Survey of Literature|journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|language=English|volume=11|pages=468|doi=10.3389/fnhum.2017.00468|pmid=28979201|pmc=5611708|issn=1662-5161}}
85. ^{{Cite journal|last=Martin|first=G. N.|last2=Clarke|first2=Richard M.|date=2017|title=Are Psychology Journals Anti-replication? A Snapshot of Editorial Practices|journal=Frontiers in Psychology|language=English|volume=8|pages=523|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00523|pmid=28443044|pmc=5387793|issn=1664-1078}}
86. ^{{Cite journal|title = Teaching Replication|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/600|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168118|pages = 600–604|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612460686|first = Michael C.|last = Frank|first2 = Rebecca|last2 = Saxe}}
87. ^{{Cite journal|title = Harnessing the Undiscovered Resource of Student Research Projects|url = http://pps.sagepub.com/content/7/6/605|journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science|date = 2012-11-01|issn = 1745-6916|pmid = 26168119|pages = 605–607|volume = 7|issue = 6|doi = 10.1177/1745691612459057|first = Jon E.|last = Grahe|first2 = Alan|last2 = Reifman|first3 = Anthony D.|last3 = Hermann|first4 = Marie|last4 = Walker|first5 = Kathryn C.|last5 = Oleson|first6 = Michelle|last6 = Nario-Redmond|first7 = Richard P.|last7 = Wiebe}}
88. ^{{Cite journal|title = Redefine statistical significance|url = https://psyarxiv.com/mky9j|journal = PsyArXiv|first = Daniel|last = Benjamin}}
89. ^{{cite journal|last1=Colquhoun|first1=David|title=An investigation of the false discovery rate and the misinterpretation of p-values|journal=Royal Society Open Science|year=2015|volume=1|issue=3|page=140216|doi=10.1098/rsos.140216|pmid=26064558|pmc=4448847|arxiv=1407.5296|bibcode=2014RSOS....140216C}}
90. ^{{cite journal|last1=Colquhoun|first1=David|title=The reproducibility of research and the misinterpretation of p-values|journal=Royal Society Open Science|volume=4|issue=12|pages=171085|date=2017|doi=10.1098/rsos.171085|pmid=29308247|pmc=5750014}}
91. ^{{cite web|last1=Colquhoun|first1=David|title=The problem with p-values|url=https://aeon.co/essays/it-s-time-for-science-to-abandon-the-term-statistically-significant|work=Aeon Magazine|accessdate=11 December 2016}}
92. ^{{cite web|last1=Longstaff|first1=Colin|last2=Colquhoun|first2=David|title=Calculator for false positive risk (FPR)|url=http://fpr-calc.ucl.ac.uk/|publisher=UCL}}
93. ^{{cite journal|last1=Matthews|first1=RAJ|title=Why should clinicians care about Bayesian methods?|journal=J. Stat. Plan. Inference|date=2001|volume=94|pages=43–58|doi=10.1016/S0378-3758(00)00232-9}}
94. ^{{Cite journal|title = Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does "failure to replicate" really mean?|journal = American Psychologist|pages = 487–498|volume = 70|issue = 6|doi = 10.1037/a0039400|first = Scott E.|last = Maxwell|first2 = Michael Y.|last2 = Lau|first3 = George S.|last3 = Howard|pmid=26348332|year = 2015}}
95. ^{{Cite journal|title = Small studies are more heterogeneous than large ones: a meta-meta-analysis|journal = Journal of Clinical Epidemiology|pages = 860–869|volume = 68|issue = 8|doi = 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.03.017|pmid = 25959635|first = Joanna|last = IntHout|first2 = John P.A.|last2 = Ioannidis|first3 = George F.|last3 = Borm|first4 = Jelle J.|last4 = Goeman|year = 2015}}
96. ^{{Cite journal|title = Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience|journal = Nature Reviews Neuroscience|date = 2013-05-01|issn = 1471-003X|pages = 365–376|volume = 14|issue = 5|doi = 10.1038/nrn3475|first = Katherine S.|last = Button|first2 = John P. A.|last2 = Ioannidis|first3 = Claire|last3 = Mokrysz|first4 = Brian A.|last4 = Nosek|first5 = Jonathan|last5 = Flint|first6 = Emma S. J.|last6 = Robinson|first7 = Marcus R.|last7 = Munafò|pmid=23571845}}
97. ^{{Cite journal|title = Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis|url = http://content.apa.org/journals/bul/82/1/1|journal = Psychological Bulletin|pages = 1–20|volume = 82|issue = 1|doi = 10.1037/h0076157|language = en-US|first = Anthony G.|last = Greenwald|year = 1975}}
98. ^{{cite journal|last1=Amrhein|first1=Valentin|last2=Korner-Nievergelt|first2=Fränzi|last3=Roth|first3=Tobias|title=The earth is flat (p > 0.05): significance thresholds and the crisis of unreplicable research|journal=PeerJ|date=2017|volume=5|page=e3544|doi=10.7717/peerj.3544|pmid=28698825|pmc=5502092}}
99. ^{{Cite journal|title = Anticipating consequences of sharing raw data and code and of awarding badges for sharing|journal = Journal of Clinical Epidemiology|volume = 70|pages = 258–60|doi = 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.04.015|pmid = 26163123|first = John P.A.|last = Ioannidis|year = 2016}}
100. ^{{cite web|title=NWO makes 3 million available for Replication Studies pilot|url=http://www.nwo.nl/en/news-and-events/news/2016/nwo-makes-3-million-available-for-replication-studies-pilot.html|website=NWO|accessdate=2 August 2016}}
101. ^{{cite news|last1=Apple|first1=Sam|title=The Young Billionaire Behind the War on Bad Science|url=https://www.wired.com/2017/01/john-arnold-waging-war-on-bad-science/|work=Wired|date=January 22, 2017}}
102. ^{{cite journal|last1=Munafò|first1=Marcus R.|last2=Smith|first2=George Davey|title=Robust research needs many lines of evidence|journal=Nature|volume=553|issue=7689|pages=399–401|date=January 23, 2018|doi=10.1038/d41586-018-01023-3|pmid=29368721}}
103. ^{{Cite journal|title = Reproducibility in Science Improving the Standard for Basic and Preclinical Research|journal = Circulation Research|date = 2015-01-02|issn = 0009-7330|pmid = 25552691|pages = 116–126|volume = 116|issue = 1|doi = 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.114.303819|first = C. Glenn|last = Begley|first2 = John P. A.|last2 = Ioannidis}}

Further reading

  • {{cite news

|newspaper=Wired
|title=The Science Behind Social Science Gets Shaken Up Again
|date=August 27, 2018
|first=Adam
|last=Rogers
|url=https://www.wired.com/story/social-science-reproducibility/}}
  • 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility, Nature.com
  • November 2012 special edition of Perspectives on Psychological Science on the issues of replicability and research practices in psychology
  • Reproducibility in Science: Improving the Standard for Basic and Preclinical Research[103]
  • [https://www.nsf.gov/sbe/AC_Materials/SBE_Robust_and_Reliable_Research_Report.pdf Social, Behavioral, and Economic Sciences Perspectives on Robust and Reliable Science]
  • Why Most Published Research Findings Are False
  • Bonett, DG (2012) Replication-extension studies. Current Directions in Psychology 21, 409-412.
  • [https://thelogicofscience.com/2016/05/10/most-scientific-studies-are-wrong-but-that-doesnt-mean-what-you-think-it-means/ Most scientific studies are wrong, but that doesn’t mean what you think it means - The Logic of Science]
  • [https://theconversation.com/science-in-crisis-from-the-sugar-scam-to-brexit-our-faith-in-experts-is-fading-65016 Article on The Conversation (website) Science in crisis: from the sugar scam to Brexit, our faith in experts is fading, September 26, 2016]
  • {{cite book |last1=Harris |first1=Richard |title=Rigor Mortis: How Sloppy Science Creates Worthless Cures, Crushes Hope, and Wastes Billions |date=2017 |publisher=Basic Books |location=New York |isbn=9780465097906}}

3 : Scientific method|Criticism of science|Ethics and statistics

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/15 11:24:49