词条 | Ambiguous image |
释义 |
The rabbit-duck image seems to be one of the earliest of this type; first published in Fliegende Blätter, a German humor magazine (Oct. 23, 1892, p. 147). Identifying and resolving ambiguous imagesMiddle vision is the stage in visual processing that combines all the basic features in the scene into distinct, recognizable object groups. This stage of vision comes before high-level vision (understanding the scene) and after early vision (determining the basic features of an image). When perceiving and recognizing images, mid-level vision comes into use when we need to classify the object we are seeing. Higher-level vision is used when the object classified must now be recognized as a specific member of its group. For example, through mid-level vision we perceive a face, then through high-level vision we recognize a face of a familiar person. Mid-level vision and high-level vision are crucial for understanding a reality that is filled with ambiguous perceptual inputs.[5] Perceiving the image in mid-level visionWhen we see an image, the first thing we do is attempt to organize all the parts of the scene into different groups.[6] To do this, one of the most basic methods used is finding the edges. Edges can include obvious perceptions such as the edge of a house, and can include other perceptions that the brain needs to process deeper, such as the edges of a person's facial features. When finding edges, the brain's visual system detects a point on the image with a sharp contrast of lighting. Being able to detect the location of the edge of an object aids in recognizing the object. In ambiguous images, detecting edges still seems natural to the person perceiving the image. However, the brain undergoes deeper processing to resolve the ambiguity. For example, consider an image that involves an opposite change in magnitude of luminance between the object and the background (e.g. From the top, the background shifts from black to white, and the object shifts from white to black). The opposing gradients will eventually come to a point where there is an equal degree of luminance of the object and the background. At this point, there is no edge to be perceived. To counter this, the visual system connects the image as a whole rather than a set of edges, allowing one to see an object rather than edges and non-edges. Although there is no complete image to be seen, the brain is able to accomplish this because of its understanding of the physical world and real incidents of ambiguous lighting.[5] In ambiguous images, an illusion is often produced from illusory contours. An illusory contour is a perceived contour without the presence of a physical gradient. In examples where a white shape appears to occlude black objects on a white background, the white shape appears to be brighter than the background, and the edges of this shape produce the illusory contours.[7] These illusory contours are processed by the brain in a similar way as real contours.[6] The visual system accomplishes this by making inferences beyond the information that is presented in much the same way as the luminance gradient. Gestalt grouping rulesIn mid-level vision, the visual system utilizes a set of heuristic methods, called Gestalt grouping rules, to quickly identify a basic perception of an object that helps to resolve an ambiguity.[2] This allows perception to be fast and easy by observing patterns and familiar images rather than a slow process of identifying each part of a group. This aids in resolving ambiguous images because the visual system will accept small variations in the pattern and still perceive the pattern as a whole. The Gestalt grouping rules are the result of the experience of the visual system. Once a pattern is perceived frequently, it is stored in memory and can be perceived again easily without the requirement of examining the entire object again.[5] For example, when looking at a chess board, we perceive a checker pattern and not a set of alternating black and white squares. Good continuationThe principle of good continuation provides the visual system a basis for identifying continuing edges. This means that when a set of lines is perceived, there is a tendency for a line to continue in one direction. This allows the visual system to identify the edges of a complex image by identifying points where lines cross. For example, two lines crossed in an "X" shape will be perceived as two lines travelling diagonally rather than two lines changing direction to form "V" shapes opposite to each other. An example of an ambiguous image would be two curving lines intersecting at a point. This junction would be perceived the same way as the "X", where the intersection is seen as the lines crossing rather than turning away from each other. Illusions of good continuation are often used by magicians to trick audiences.[8] SimilarityThe rule of similarity states that images that are similar to each other can be grouped together as being the same type of object or part of the same object. Therefore, the more similar two images or objects are, the more likely it will be that they can be grouped together. For example, two squares among many circles will be grouped together. They can vary in similarity of colour, size, orientation and other properties, but will ultimately be grouped together with varying degrees of membership.[5] Proximity, common region, and connectednessThe grouping property of proximity (Gestalt) is the spatial distance between two objects. The closer two objects are, the more likely they belong to the same group. This perception can be ambiguous without the person perceiving it as ambiguous. For example, two objects with varying distances and orientations from the viewer may appear to be proximal to each other, while a third object may be closer to one of the other objects but appear farther. Objects occupying a common region on the image appear to already be members of the same group. This can include unique spatial location, such as two objects occupying a distinct region of space outside of their group's own. Objects can have close proximity but appear as though part of a distinct group through various visual aids such as a threshold of colours separating the two objects. Additionally, objects can be visually connected in ways such as drawing a line going from each object. These similar but hierarchical rules suggest that some Gestalt rules can override other rules.[5] Texture segmentation and figure-ground assignmentsThe visual system can also aid itself in resolving ambiguities by detecting the pattern of texture in an image. This is accomplished by using many of the Gestalt principles. The texture can provide information that helps to distinguish whole objects, and the changing texture in an image reveals which distinct objects may be part of the same group. Texture segmentation rules often both cooperate and compete with each other, and examining the texture can yield information about the layers of the image, disambiguating the background, foreground, and the object.[9] Size and surroundednessWhen a region of texture completely surrounds another region of texture, it is likely the background. Additionally, the smaller regions of texture in an image are likely the figure.[5] Parallelism and symmetryParallelism is another way to disambiguate the figure of an image. The orientation of the contours of different textures in an image can determine which objects are grouped together. Generally, parallel contours suggest membership to the same object or group of objects. Similarly, symmetry of the contours can also define the figure of an image.[5]Extremal edges and relative motionAn extremal edge is a change in texture that suggests an object is in front of or behind another object. This can be due to a shading effect on the edges of one region of texture, giving the appearance of depth. Some extremal edge effects can overwhelm the segmentations of surroundedness or size. The edges perceived can also aid in distinguishing objects by examining the change in texture against an edge due to motion.[5] Using ambiguous images to hide in the real world: camouflageIn nature, camouflage is used by organisms to escape predators. This is achieved through creating an ambiguity of texture segmentation by imitating the surrounding environment. Without being able to perceive noticeable differences in texture and position, a predator will be unable to see their victim.[5] OcclusionMany ambiguous images are produced through some occlusion, wherein an object's texture suddenly stops. An occlusion is the visual perception of one object being behind or in front of another object, providing information about the order of the layers of texture.[5] The illusion of occlusion is apparent in the effect of illusory contours, where occlusion is perceived despite being non-existent. Here, an ambiguous image is perceived to be an instance of occlusion. When an object is occluded, the visual system only has information about the parts of the object that can be seen, so the rest of the processing must be done deeper and must involve memory. Accidental viewpointsAn accidental viewpoint is a single visual position that produces an ambiguous image. The accidental viewpoint does not provide enough information to distinguish what the object is.[10] Often, this image is perceived incorrectly and produces an illusion that differs from reality. For example, an image may be split in half, with the top half being enlarged and placed further away from the perceiver in space. This image will be perceived as one complete image from only a single viewpoint in space, rather than the reality of two separate halves of an object. Street artists often use tricks of point-of-view to create 2-dimensional scenes on the ground that appear 3-dimensional. Recognizing an object through high-level visionTo go further than just perceiving the object is to recognize the object. Recognizing an object plays a crucial role in resolving ambiguous images, and relies heavily on memory and prior knowledge. To recognize an object, the visual system detects familiar components of it, and compares the perceptual representation of it with a representation of the object stored in memory.[6] This can be done using various templates of an object, such as "dog" to represent dogs in general. The template method is not always successful because members of a group may significantly differ visually from each other, and may look much different if viewed from different angles. To counter the problem of viewpoint, the visual system detects familiar components of an object in 3-dimensional space. If the components of an object perceived are in the same position and orientation of an object in memory, recognition is possible.[5] Research has shown that people that are more creative in their imagery are better able to resolve ambiguous images. This may be due to their ability to quickly identify patterns in the image.[11] When making a mental representation of an ambiguous image, in the same way as normal images, each part is defined and then put onto the mental representation. The more complex the scene is, the longer it takes to process and add to the representation.[12] Figures drawn in a way that avoids depth cues may become ambiguous. Classic examples of this phenomenon are the Necker cube,[5] and the rhombille tiling (viewed as an isometric drawing of cubes). Using memory and recent experienceOur memory has a large impact on resolving an ambiguous image, as it helps the visual system to identify and recognize objects without having to analyze and categorize them repeatedly. Without memory and prior knowledge, an image with several groups of similar objects will be difficult to perceive. Any object can have an ambiguous representation and can be mistakenly categorized into the wrong groups without sufficient memory recognition of an object. This finding suggests that prior experience is necessary for proper perception.[13] Studies have been done with the use of Greebles to show the role of memory in object recognition.[5] The act of priming the participant with an exposure to a similar visual stimulus also has a large effect on the ease of resolving an ambiguity.[13] Disorders in perceptionProsopagnosia is a disorder that causes a person to be unable to identify faces. The visual system undergoes mid-level vision and identifies a face, but high-level vision fails to identify who the face belongs to. In this case, the visual system identifies an ambiguous object, a face, but is unable to resolve the ambiguity using memory, leaving the affected unable to determine who they are seeing.[5]See also
References1. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Parkkonen | first1 = L. | last2 = Andersson | first2 = J. | last3 = Hämäläinen | first3 = M. | last4 = Hari | first4 = R. | year = 2008 | title = Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 105 | issue = 51| pages = 20500–20504 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0810966105 | pmid=19074267 | pmc=2602606}} 2. ^1 {{cite journal | last1 = Wimmer | first1 = M. | last2 = Doherty | first2 = M. | year = 2011 | title = The development of ambiguous figure perception: Vi. conception and perception of ambiguous figures | url = | journal = Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development | volume = 76 | issue = 1| pages = 87–104 | doi = 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00595.x }} 3. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Mast | first1 = F.W. | last2 = Kosslyn | first2 = S.M. | year = 2002 | title = Visual mental images can be ambiguous: Insights from individual differences in spatial transformation abilities | url = | journal = Cognition | volume = 86 | issue = 1| pages = 57–70 | doi = 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00137-3 }} 4. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Chambers | first1 = D. | last2 = Reisberg | first2 = D. | year = 1985 | title = Can mental images be ambiguous? | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | volume = 11 | issue = 3| pages = 317–328 | doi = 10.1037/0096-1523.11.3.317 }} 5. ^1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Wolfe, J., Kluender, K., & Levi, D. (2009). Sensation and perception. (2 ed.). Sunderland: Sinauer Associates. 6. ^1 2 Halko, M.A. Illusory contour and surface completion mechanisms in human visual cortex. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 3303. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/621754807 7. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Bradley | first1 = D.R. | last2 = Dumais | first2 = S.T. | year = 1975 | title = Ambiguous cognitive contours | url = | journal = Nature | volume = 257 | issue = 5527| pages = 582–584 | doi = 10.1038/257582a0 }} 8. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Bamhart | first1 = A.S. | year = 2010 | title = The exploitation of gestalt principles by magicians | url = | journal = Perception | volume = 39 | issue = 9| pages = 1286–1289 | doi = 10.1068/p6766 }} 9. ^Tang, X.A model for figure-ground segmentation by self-organized cue integration. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 3245. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/621577763 10. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Koning | first1 = A. | last2 = van Lier | first2 = R. | year = 2006 | title = No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints | url = | journal = Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung | volume = 70 | issue = 1| pages = 52–58 | doi = 10.1007/s00426-004-0191-8 }} 11. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Riquelme | first1 = H | year = 2002 | title = Can people creative in imagery interpret ambiguous figures faster than people less creative in imagery? | url = | journal = The Journal of Creative Behavior | volume = 36 | issue = 2| pages = 105–116 | doi = 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2002.tb01059.x }} 12. ^{{cite journal | last1 = Kosslyn | first1 = S.M. | last2 = Reiser | first2 = B.J. | last3 = Farah | first3 = M.J. | last4 = Fliegel | first4 = S.L. | year = 1983 | title = Generating visual images: Units and relations | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | volume = 112 | issue = 2| pages = 278–303 | doi = 10.1037/0096-3445.112.2.278 }} 13. ^1 {{cite journal | last1 = Daelli | first1 = V. | last2 = van Rijsbergen | first2 = N.J. | last3 = Treves | first3 = A. | year = 2010 | title = How recent experience affects the perception of ambiguous objects | url = | journal = Brain Research | volume = 1322 | issue = | pages = 81–91 | doi = 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.060 | pmid=20122901}} External links{{commons category|Reversible figure}}
2 : Perception|Optical illusions |
随便看 |
|
开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。