请输入您要查询的百科知识:

 

词条 Italian phonology
释义

  1. Consonants

  2. Vowels

  3. Phonotactics

      Onset    Nucleus    Coda  

  4. Syntactic gemination

  5. Regional variation

  6. Phonological development

      Phoneme inventory    17 months    21 months    24 months    27 months    Phonotactics    Syllable structures    6–10 months    11–14 months    18–24 months    Stress patterns    Phonological awareness  

  7. Sample texts

  8. See also

  9. References

  10. Bibliography

  11. External links

{{selfref|For assistance with IPA transcriptions of Italian for Wikipedia articles, see IPA/Italian.}}{{main|Italian language}}{{Refimprove|date=July 2012}}{{IPA notice}}{{Italian language|state=expanded}}

The phonology of Italian describes the sound system—the phonology and phonetics—of Standard Italian and its geographical variants.

{{TOC limit|limit=3}}

Consonants

Consonant phonemes
LabialDental/
alveolar
Post-
alveolar/
palatal
Velar
Nasal{{IPA link|m}}{{IPA link|n}} {{IPA link|ɲ}}
Stop{{IPA link|p}}{{IPA link|b}}{{IPA link|t̪|t}}{{IPA link|d̪|d}}{{IPA link|k}}{{IPA link|ɡ}}
Affricate{{IPA link|t͡s}}{{IPA link|d͡z}}{{IPA link|t͡ʃ}}{{IPA link|d͡ʒ}}
Fricative{{IPA link|f}}{{IPA link|v}}{{IPA link|s}}{{IPA link|z}}{{IPA link|ʃ}}
Approximant {{IPA link|j}}{{IPA link|w}}
Lateral{{IPA link|l}} {{IPA link|ʎ}}
Trill{{IPA link|r}}

Notes:

  • Between two vowels, or between a vowel and an approximant ({{IPA|/j, w/}}) or a liquid ({{IPA|/l, r/}}), consonants can be both singleton or geminated. Geminated consonants shorten the preceding vowel (or block phonetic lengthening) and the first geminated element is unreleased. For example, compare {{IPA|/fato/}} {{IPA|[ˈfaːto]}} ('fate') with {{IPA|/fatto/}} {{IPA|[ˈfatto]}} ('fact'). However, {{IPA|/ɲɲ/}}, {{IPA|/ʃʃ/}}, {{IPA|/ʎʎ/}}, are always geminated word-internally.{{sfnp|Hall|1944|pp=77–78}} Similarly, nasals, liquids, and sibilants are pronounced slightly longer in medial consonant clusters.{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=78}}
  • {{IPA|/j/}}, {{IPA|/w/}}, and {{IPA|/z/}} are the only consonants that cannot be geminated.
  • {{IPA|/t, d/}} are laminal denti-alveolar {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|t̪}}, {{IPAplink|d̪}}]}},{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=132}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|p=62}}{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=117}} commonly called "dental" for simplicity.
  • {{IPA|/k, ɡ/}} are pre-velar before {{IPA|/i, e, ɛ, j/}}.{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|p=62}}
  • {{IPA|/t͡s, d͡z, s, z/}} have two variants:
    • Dentalized laminal alveolar {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|t̪͡s̪}}, {{IPAplink|d̪͡z̪}}, {{IPAplink|s̪}}, {{IPAplink|z̪}}]}}{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=132}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=68 and 75–76}} (commonly called "dental" for simplicity), pronounced with the blade of the tongue very close to the upper front teeth, with the tip of the tongue resting behind lower front teeth.{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=68 and 75–76}}
    • Non-retracted apical alveolar {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|t͡s̺}}, {{IPAplink|d͡z̺}}, {{IPAplink|s̺}}, {{IPAplink|z̺}}]}}.{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=68 and 75–76}} The stop components of the "apical" affricates is actually laminal denti-alveolar.{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=68 and 75–76}}
  • {{IPA|/n, l, r/}} are apical alveolar {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|n|n̺}}, {{IPAplink|l|l̺}}, {{IPAplink|r|r̺}}]}} in most environments.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=132}}{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=117}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=57, 84 and 88–89}} The first two are pronounced as laminal denti-alveolar {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|n̪}}, {{IPAplink|l̪}}]}} before {{IPA|/t, d, t͡s, d͡z, s, z/}}{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=117}}{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=133}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=58 and 88–89}} and palatalized laminal postalveolar {{IPA|[{{IPA|n̠ʲ}}, {{IPA|l̠ʲ}}]}} before {{IPA|/t͡ʃ, d͡ʒ ʃ/}}.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=134}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|pp=57–59 and 88–89}} {{IPA|/n/}} has a velar allophone {{IPAblink|ŋ}} before {{IPA|/k, ɡ/}}.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|pp=134–135}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|p=59}}
  • {{IPA|/m/}} and {{IPA|/n/}} do not contrast before {{IPA|/p, b/}} and {{IPA|/f, v/}}, where they are pronounced {{IPAblink|m}} and {{IPAblink|ɱ}}, respectively.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|pp=134–135}}{{sfnp|Canepari|1992|p=58}}
  • {{IPA|/ɲ/}} and {{IPA|/ʎ/}} are alveolo-palatal.{{sfnp|Recasens|2013|p=222}} In a large number of accents, {{IPA|/ʎ/}} is a fricative {{IPAblink|ʎ̝}}.[1]
  • Some accents from central Italy and southern Italy, such as the Roman, do not have the {{IPA|/ʎ/}} sound; instead, it is pronounced as {{IPA|[j]}}, or, sometimes, {{IPA|[ʝ]}}.{{citation needed|date=January 2019}}
  • In almost all speeches from northern Italy, geminates are not realised.
  • Intervocalically, single {{IPA|/r/}} is realised as a trill with one or two contacts.{{sfnp|Ladefoged|Maddieson|1996|p=221}} Some literature treats the single-contact trill as a tap {{IPAblink|ɾ}}.{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=118}}[2] Single-contact trills can also occur elsewhere, particularly in unstressed syllables.[3] Geminate {{IPA|/rr/}} manifests as a trill with three to seven contacts.{{sfnp|Ladefoged|Maddieson|1996|p=221}}
  • The phonetic distinction between {{IPA|[s]}} and {{IPA|[z]}} is neutralized before consonants and at the beginning of words: the former is used before voiceless consonants and before vowels at the beginning of words; the latter is used before voiced consonants (meaning {{IPA|[z]}} is an allophone of {{IPA|/s/}} before voiced consonants). The two can contrast only between vowels within a word, e.g. [ˈfuːzo] 'melted' vs. [ˈfuːso] 'spindle'. According to Canepari,[2] though, the traditional standard has been replaced by a modern neutral pronunciation which always prefers {{IPA|/z/}} when intervocalic, except when the intervocalic s is the initial sound of a word, if the compound is still felt as such: for example, presento {{IPA|/preˈsɛnto/}}[4] ('I foresee', with pre meaning 'before' and sento meaning 'I see') vs presento {{IPA|/preˈzɛnto/}}[5] ('I present'). There are many words in which dictionaries now indicate that both pronunciations with {{IPA|[z]}} and with {{IPA|[s]}} are acceptable. Word-internally between vowels, the two phonemes have merged in many regional varieties of Italian, either as {{IPA|/z/}} (Northern-Central) or {{IPA|/s/}} (Southern-Central).

Vowels

Vowel phonemes
Front Central Back
Closei}}u}}
Close-mide}}o}}
Open-midɛ}}ɔ}}
Openä|a}}

In Italian there is no phonemic distinction between long and short vowels, but vowels in stressed open syllables, unless word-final, are long at the end of the intonational phrase (including isolated words) or when emphasized.{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=119}}{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=136}} Adjacent identical vowels found at morpheme boundaries are not resyllabified, but pronounced separately ("quickly rearticulated"), and they might be reduced to a single short vowel in rapid speech.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=137}}

Although Italian contrasts close-mid ({{IPA|/e, o/}}) and open-mid ({{IPA|/ɛ, ɔ/}}) vowels in stressed syllables, this distinction is neutralised in unstressed position,{{sfnp|Rogers|d'Arcangeli|2004|p=119}} where only the close-mid vowels occur. The height of these vowels in unstressed position is context-sensitive; they are somewhat lowered ({{IPA|[{{IPAplink|e̞}}, {{IPAplink|o̞}}]}}) in the vicinity of more open vowels.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|pp=137–138}} The distinction between close-mid and open-mid vowels is lost entirely in a few Southern varieties, especially in the Northern Sicilian dialect (Palermo), where they are realized as open-mid {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|ɛ}}, {{IPAplink|ɔ}}]}}, as well as in some Northern varieties (in particular in Piedmont), where they are realized as mid {{IPA|[{{IPAplink|e̞}}, {{IPAplink|o̞}}]}}.

Word-final stressed {{IPA|/ɔ/}} is found in a small number of words: però, ciò, gio’ (short for gioia)[6], and the first person singular future of all verbs and the third person singular preterite of most verbs. Word-final unstressed {{IPA|/u/}} is rare.{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=138}} Major exceptions are onomatopoeic terms (babau),[7] loanwords (guru),[8] and place or family names derived from the Sardinian language (Gennargentu,[9] Porcu).[10]

When the last phoneme of a word is an unstressed vowel and the first phoneme of the following word is any vowel, the former vowel tends to become non-syllabic. This phenomenon is called synalepha and should be taken into account when counting syllables, e.g. in poetry.

In addition to monophthongs, Italian has diphthongs, but these are both phonemically and phonetically simply combinations of the other vowels, with some being very common (e.g. {{IPA|/ai, au/}}), others being rarer (e.g. {{IPA|/ɛi/}}) and some never occurring within (native) Italian words (e.g. {{IPA|/ou/}}). None of these diphthongs are however considered to have distinct phonemic status because their constituents do not behave differently than they would in isolation (and all occur in isolation), unlike the diphthongs in some languages like English and German. Grammatical tradition makes a distinction between ‘falling’ and ‘rising’ diphthongs; however, since rising diphthongs are composed of one semiconsonantal sound {{IPA|[j]}} or {{IPA|[w]}} and one vowel sound, they are not actually diphthongs. The practice of referring to them as ‘diphthongs’ has been criticised by phoneticians like Luciano Canepari.[2]

Phonotactics

Onset

Italian allows up to three consonants in syllable-initial position, though there are limitations:{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=79}}

CC
  • {{IPA|/s/}} + any voiceless stop or {{IPA|/f/}}. E.g. spavento ('fright')
  • {{IPA|/z/}} + any voiced stop, {{IPA|/v d͡ʒ m n l r/}}. E.g. srotolare ('unroll')
  • {{IPA|/f v/}}, or any stop + {{IPA|/r/}}. E.g. frana ('landslide')
  • {{IPA|/f v/}}, or any stop except {{IPA|/t d/}} + {{IPA|/l/}}. E.g. platano ('planetree')
  • {{IPA|/f v s z/}}, or any stop or nasal + {{IPA|/j w/}}. E.g. fiume ('river'), vuole ('he/she wants'), siamo ('we are'), suono ('sound')
  • In words of foreign (mostly Greek) origin which are only partially assimilated, other combinations such as {{IPA|/pn/}} (e.g. pneumatico), {{IPA|/mn/}} (e.g. mnemonico), {{IPA|/tm/}} (e.g. tmesi), and {{IPA|/ps/}} (e.g. pseudo-) occur.

As an onset, the cluster {{IPA|/s/}} + voiceless consonant is inherently unstable. Phonetically, word-internal s+C normally syllabifies as {{IPA|[s.C]:}} {{IPA|[ˈrɔs.po]}} rospo 'toad', {{IPA|[tras.ˈteː.ve.re]}} Trastevere (neighborhood of Rome).[11]{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=80}} Phonetic syllabification of the cluster also occurs at word boundaries if a vowel precedes it without pause, e.g. {{IPA|[las.ˈtɔː.rja]}} la storia 'the history', implying the same syllable break at the structural level, {{IPA|/sˈtɔrja/}},[12] thus always latent due to the extrasyllabic {{IPA|/s/}}, but unrealized phonetically unless a vowel precedes.[13] A competing analysis accepts that while the syllabification {{IPA|/s.C/}} is accurate historically, modern retreat of i-prosthesis before word initial {{IPA|/s/}}+C (e.g. erstwhile con isforzo 'with effort' has generally given way to con sforzo) suggests that the structure is now underdetermined, with occurrence of {{IPA|/s.C/}} or {{IPA|/.sC/}} variable "according to the context and the idiosyncratic behaviour of the speakers."{{sfnp|Bertinetto|Loporcaro|2005|p=141}}

CCC
  • {{IPA|/s/}} + voiceless stop or {{IPA|/f/}} + {{IPA|/r/}}. E.g. spregiare ('to despise')
  • {{IPA|/s/}} + {{IPA|/p k/}} + {{IPA|/l/}}. E.g. sclerosi ('sclerosis')
  • {{IPA|/z/}} + voiced stop + {{IPA|/r/}}. E.g. sbracciato ('with bare arms'), sdraiare ('to lay down'), sgravare ('to relieve')
  • {{IPA|/z/}} + {{IPA|/b/}} + {{IPA|/l/}}. E.g. sbloccato ('unblocked')
  • {{IPA|/f v/}} or any stop + {{IPA|/r/}} + {{IPA|/j w/}}. E.g. priego (antiquated form of prego 'I pray'), proprio ('(one's) own' / proper / properly), pruovo (antiquated form of provo 'I try')
  • {{IPA|/f v/}} or any stop or nasal + {{IPA|/w/}} + {{IPA|/j/}}. E.g. quieto ('quiet'), continuiamo ('we continue')

The last combination is however rare and one of the approximants is often vocalised, e.g. quieto {{IPA|/ˈkwjɛto, kwiˈɛto/}}, continuiamo {{IPA|/((kontiˈnwjamo)), kontinuˈjamo, kontinwiˈamo/}}

Nucleus

The nucleus is the only mandatory part of a syllable (for instance, a 'to, at' is a word) and must be a vowel or a diphthong. In a falling diphthong the most common second elements are {{IPA|/i̯/}} or {{IPA|/u̯/}} but other combinations such as idea {{IPA|/iˈdɛa̯/}}, trae {{IPA|/ˈtrae̯/}} may also be interpreted as diphthongs.[2] Combinations of {{IPA|/j w/}} with vowels are often labelled diphthongs, allowing for combinations of {{IPA|/j w/}} with falling diphthongs to be called triphthongs. One view holds that it is more accurate to label {{IPA|/j w/}} as consonants and {{IPA|/jV wV/}} as consonant-vowel sequences rather than rising diphthongs. In that interpretation, Italian has only falling diphthongs (phonemically at least, cf. Synaeresis) and no triphthongs.[2]

Coda

A coda is permissible only in the case of monophthong nuclei {{Citation needed|date=June 2017}}, and can be one of:

  • {{IPA|/r/}}. E.g. per ('for'), parte ('part')
  • {{IPA|/l/}}. E.g. al ('to the'), alto ('high')
  • {{IPA|/n/}}. E.g. con ('with'), conto ('count'), which undergoes assimilation if a consonantal onset follows, e.g. un poco {{IPA|[umˈpɔːko]}} ('a little')
  • {{IPA|/m/}}. E.g. compro ('I buy'), ampio ('ample')
  • the first element of any geminate, e.g. tutto ('everything'), avvertire ('to warn')

Syntactic gemination

{{main|Syntactic gemination}}

Word-initial consonants are geminated after certain vowel-final words in the same prosodic unit. There are two types of triggers of initial gemination: some unstressed particles, prepositions, and other monosyllabic words, and any oxytonic polysyllabic word.{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=80}} As an example of the first type, casa ('house') is pronounced {{IPA|[ˈkaːsa]}} but a casa ('homeward') is pronounced {{IPA|[akˈkaːsa]}}. This is not a purely phonological process, as no gemination is cued by the la in la casa 'the house' {{IPA|[laˈkaːsa]}}, and there is nothing detectable in the structure of the preposition a to account for the gemination. This type normally originates in language history: modern a, for example, derives from Latin AD, and today's geminate in {{IPA|[akˈkaːsa]}} is a continuation of what was once a simple assimilation. Gemination cued by final stressed vowels, however, is transparently phonological. Final stressed vowels are short by nature, if a consonant follows a short stressed vowel the syllable must be closed, thus the consonant following the final stressed vowel is drawn to lengthen: parlò portoghese {{IPA|[parˈlɔpportoˈɡeːze]}} 's/he spoke Portuguese' vs. parla portoghese {{IPA|[ˈparlaportoˈɡeːze]}} 's/he speaks Portuguese'.

Syntactic gemination is the normal native pronunciation in Central Italy (both "stress-induced" and "lexical") and Southern Italy (only "lexical"), including Sicily and Corsica (France).

In Northern Italy and Sardinia, speakers use it inconsistently because the feature is not present in the dialectal substratum and is not usually shown in the written language unless a new word is produced by the fusion of the two: "chi sa"-> chissà ("who knows" in the sense of goodness knows).

Regional variation

{{main|Regional Italian}}

The above IPA symbols and description refer to standard Italian, based on a somewhat idealized version of the Tuscan-derived national language. As is common in many cultures, this single version of the language was pushed as neutral, proper, and eventually superior, leading to some stigmatization of varying accents. Television news anchors and other high-profile figures had to put aside their regional Italian when in the public sphere. However, in more recent years the enforcement of this standard has fallen out of favor in Italy, and news reporters, actors, and the like are now more free to deliver their words in their native regional variety of Italian, which appeals to the Italian population's range of linguistic diversity. The variety is still not represented in its wholeness and accents from the South are maybe to be considered less popular, except in shows set in the South and in comedy, a field in which Naples, Sicily and the South in general have always been present. Though it still represents the basics for the standard variety, the loosened restrictions have led to Tuscan being seen for what it is, just one dialect among many with its own regional peculiarities and qualities, many of which are shared with Umbria, Southern Marche and Northern Lazio.

  • In Tuscany (though not in standard Italian, which is derived from, but not equivalent to, Tuscan dialect), voiceless stops are typically pronounced as fricatives between vowels.{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=75}} That is, {{IPA|/p t k/}} → {{IPA|[ɸ θ h/x]}}: e.g. i capitani 'the captains' {{IPA|[iˌhaɸiˈθaːni]}}, a phenomenon known as the gorgia toscana 'Tuscan throat'. In a much more widespread area of Central Italy, postalveolar affricates are deaffricated when intervocalic so that in Cina ('in China') is pronounced {{IPA|[in t͡ʃiːna]}} but la Cina ('the China') is {{IPA|[laʃiːna]}}, and /ˈbat͡ʃo/ bacio 'kiss' is [ˈbaːʃo] rather than Standard Italian [ˈbaːt͡ʃo].{{sfnp|Hall|1944|p=76}} This deaffrication can result in minimal pairs distinguished only by length of the fricatives, [ʃ] issuing from /t͡ʃ/ and [ʃː] from geminate /ʃʃ/: {{IPA|[laʃeˈrɔ]}} lacerò 's/he ripped' vs. {{IPA|[laʃːeˈrɔ]}} lascerò 'I will leave'.
  • In nonstandard varieties of Central and Southern Italian, some stops at the end of a syllable completely assimilate to the following consonant.{{Citation needed|date=January 2009}} For example, a Venetian might say tecnica as {{IPA|[ˈtɛknika]}} or {{IPA|[ˈtɛɡnika]}} in violation of normal Italian consonant contact restrictions{{Clarify|date=December 2012}}, while a Florentine would likely pronounce tecnica as {{IPA|[ˈtɛnniha]}}, a Roman on a range from {{IPA|[ˈtɛnnika]}} to {{IPA|[ˈtɛnniɡa]}} (in Southern Italian, complex clusters usually are separated by a vowel: a Neapolitan would say {{IPA|[ˈtɛkkənikə]}}, a Sicilian {{IPA|[ˈtɛkkɪnɪka]}}). Similarly, although the cluster {{IPA|/kt/}} has developed historically as {{IPA|/tt/}} through assimilation, a learned word such as ictus will be pronounced {{IPA|[ittus]}} by some, {{IPA|[iktus]}} by others.
  • In popular (non-Tuscan) Central and Southern Italian speech, {{IPA|/b/}} and {{IPA|/d͡ʒ/}} tend to always be geminated ({{IPA|[bb]}} and {{IPA|[dd͡ʒ]}}) when between two vowels, or a vowel and a sonorant ({{IPA|/j/}}, {{IPA|/w/}}, {{IPA|/l/}}, or {{IPA|/r/}}). Sometimes this is also used in written language, e.g. writing robba instead of roba ('property'), to suggest a regional accent, though this spelling is considered incorrect. In Tuscany and beyond in Central and Southern Italy, intervocalic non-geminate {{IPA|/d͡ʒ/}} is realized as {{IPA|[ʒ]}} (parallel to {{IPA|/t͡ʃ/}} realized as {{IPA|[ʃ]}} described above).
  • The two phonemes {{IPA|/s/}} and {{IPA|/z/}} have merged in many varieties of Italian: when between two vowels within the same word, it tends to always be pronounced {{IPA|[z]}} in Northern Italy, and {{IPA|[s]}} in Central and Southern Italy (except in the Arbëreshë community). A notable example is the word casa ('house'): in Northern Italy it is pronounced {{IPA|[ˈkaːza]}}; in Southern-Central Italy it's pronounced {{IPA|[ˈkaːsa]}}.
  • In several Southern varieties, voiceless stops tend to be voiced if following a sonorant, as an influence of the still largely spoken regional languages: campo {{IPA|/ˈkampo/}} is often pronounced {{IPA|[ˈkambo]}}, and Antonio {{IPA|/anˈtɔnjo/}} is frequently {{IPA|[anˈdɔnjo]}}.

The various Tuscan, Corsican and Central Italian dialects are, to some extent, the closest ones to Standard Italian in terms of linguistic features, since the latter is based on a somewhat polished form of Florentine.

Phonological development

{{main|Phonological development}}

Very little research has been done on the earliest stages of phonological development in Italian.{{sfnp|Keren-Portnoy|Majorano|Vihman|2009|p=240}} This article primarily describes phonological development after the first year of life. See the main article on phonological development for a description of first year stages. Many of the earliest stages are thought to be universal to all infants.

Phoneme inventory

Word-final consonants are rarely produced during the early stages of word production. Consonants are usually found in word-initial position, or in intervocalic position.{{sfnp|Zmarich|Bonifacio|2005|p=759}}

17 months

Most consonants are word-initial: They are the stops {{IPA|/p/}}, {{IPA|/b/}}, {{IPA|/t/}}, and {{IPA|/k/}} and the nasal {{IPA|/m/}}. A preference for a front place of articulation is present.

21 months

More phones now appear in intervocalic contexts. The additions to the phonetic inventory are the voiced stop {{IPA|/d/}}, the nasal {{IPA|/n/}}, the voiceless affricate {{IPA|/t͡ʃ/}}, and the liquid {{IPA|/l/}}.{{sfnp|Zmarich|Bonifacio|2005|p=759}}

24 months

The fricatives {{IPA|/f/}}, {{IPA|/v/}}, and {{IPA|/s/}} are added, primarily at the intervocalic position.{{sfnp|Zmarich|Bonifacio|2005|p=759}}

27 months

Approximately equal numbers of phones are now produced in word-initial and intervocalic position. Additions to the phonetic inventory are the voiced stop {{IPA|/ɡ/}} and the consonant cluster {{IPA|/kw/}}. While the word-initial inventory now tends to have all the phones of the adult targets (adult production of the child's words), the intervocalic inventory tends to still be missing four consonants or consonant clusters of the adult targets: {{IPA|/f/}}, {{IPA|/d͡ʒ/}}, {{IPA|/r/}}, and {{IPA|/st/}}.{{sfnp|Zmarich|Bonifacio|2005|p=759}}

Stops are the most common manner of articulation at all stages and are produced more often than they are present in the target words at around 18 months. Gradually this frequency decreases to almost target-like frequency by around 27 months. The opposite process happens with fricatives, affricates, laterals and trills. Initially, the production of these phonemes is significantly less than what is found in the target words and the production continues to increases to target-like frequency. Alveolars and bilabials are the two most common places of articulation, with alveolar production steadily increasing after the first stage and bilabial production gently decreasing. Labiodental and postalveolar production increases throughout development, while velar production decreases.{{sfnp|Zmarich|Bonifacio|2005|p=760}}

Phonotactics

Syllable structures

6–10 months

Babbling becomes distinct from previous, less structured vocal play. Initially, syllable structure is limited to CVCV, called reduplicated babbling. At this stage, children’s vocalizations have a weak relation to adult Italian and the Italian lexicon.{{sfnp|Majorano|D'Odorico|2011|p=53}}

11–14 months

The most-used syllable type changes as children age, and the distribution of syllables takes on increasingly Italian characteristics. This ability significantly increases between the ages of 11 and 12 months, 12 and 13 months, and 13 and 14 months.{{sfnp|Majorano|D'Odorico|2011|p=53}} Consonant clusters are still absent. Children's first ten words appear around month 12, and take CVCV format (e.g. mama 'mother', papa 'father').{{sfnp|Fasolo|Majorano|D'Odorico|2006|p=86}}

18–24 months

Reduplicated babbling is replaced by variegated babbling, producing syllable structures such as C1VC2V (e.g. cane 'dog', topo 'mouse'). Production of trisyllabic words begins (e.g. pecora 'sheep', matita 'pencil').{{sfnp|Fasolo|Majorano|D'Odorico|2006|p=86}} Consonant clusters are now present (e.g. bimba 'female child', venti 'twenty'). Ambient language plays an increasingly significant role as children begin to solidify early syllable structure. Syllable combinations that are infrequent in the Italian lexicon, such as velar-labial sequences (e.g. capra 'goat' or gamba 'leg') are infrequently produced correctly by children, and are often subject to consonant harmony.{{sfnp|Majorano|D'Odorico|2011|p=58}}

Stress patterns

In Italian, stress is lexical, meaning it is word-specific and partly unpredictable. Penultimate stress (primary stress on the second-to-last syllable) is also generally preferred.[14]{{sfnp|D'imperio|Rosenthall|1999|p=5}} This goal, acting simultaneously with the child's initial inability to produce polysyllabic words, often results in weak-syllable deletion. The primary environment for weak-syllable deletion in polysyllabic words is word-initial, as deleting word-final or word-medial syllables would interfere with the penultimate stress pattern heard in ambient language.{{sfnp|Majorano|D'Odorico|2011|p=61}}

Phonological awareness

Children develop syllabic segmentation awareness earlier than phonemic segmentation awareness. In earlier stages, syllables are perceived as a separate phonetic unit, while phonemes are perceived as assimilated units by coarticulation in spoken language. By first grade, Italian children are nearing full development of segmentation awareness on both syllables and phonemes. Compared to those children whose mother tongue exhibits closed syllable structure (CVC,CCVC, CVCC, etc.), Italian-speaking children develop this segmentation awareness earlier, possibly due to its open syllable structure (CVCV, CVCVCV, etc.).{{sfnp|Cossu|Shankweiler|Liberman|Katz|1988|p=10}} Rigidity in Italian (shallow orthography and open syllable structure) makes it easier for Italian-speaking children to be aware of those segments.{{sfnp|Cossu|Shankweiler|Liberman|Katz|1988|p=11}}

Sample texts

{{Listen
| filename = It-Vangeli.ogg
| title = Milan speaker reading the Bible, Luke 2, 1–7.
}}

Provided here is a rendition of the Bible, Luke 2, 1–7, as read by a native Italian speaker from Milan. As a northerner, his pronunciation lacks syntactic doubling ({{IPA|[ˈfu ˈfatto]}} instead of {{IPA|[ˈfu fˈfatto]}}) and intervocalic {{IPA|[s]}} ({{IPA|[ˈkaːza]}} instead of {{IPA|[ˈkaːsa]}}). The speaker realises {{IPA|/r/}} as {{IPA|[ʋ]}} in some positions.

2:1 In quei giorni, un decreto di Cesare Augusto ordinava che si facesse un censimento di tutta la terra.
2 Questo primo censimento fu fatto quando Quirino era governatore della Siria.
3 Tutti andavano a farsi registrare, ciascuno nella propria città.
4 Anche Giuseppe, che era della casa e della famiglia di Davide, dalla città di Nazaret e dalla Galilea si recò in Giudea nella città di Davide, chiamata Betlemme,
5 per farsi registrare insieme a Maria, sua sposa, che era incinta.
6 Proprio mentre si trovavano lì, venne il tempo per lei di partorire.
7 Mise al mondo il suo primogenito, lo avvolse in fasce e lo depose in una mangiatoia, poiché non c'era posto per loro nella locanda.

The differences in pronunciation are underlined in the following transcriptions; the velar {{IPA|[ŋ]}} is an allophone of {{IPA|/n/}} and the long vowels are allophones of the short vowels, but are shown for clarity.

A rough transcription of the audio sample is:

2:1 {{

IPA|[iŋ ˈkwɛi ˈdʒorni un deˈkreːto di ˈtʃeːzare auˈɡusto ordiˈnaːva ke si faˈtʃɛsse un tʃensiˈmento di ˈtutta la ˈtɛrra}}
2 {{

IPA|ˈkwɛsto ˈpriːmo tʃensiˈmento fu ˈfatto ˈkwando kwiˈriːno ˈeːra ɡovernaˈtoːre dɛlla ˈsiːrja}}
3 {{

IPA|ˈtutti anˈdaːvano a ˈfarsi redʒiˈstraːre tʃaˈskuːno nɛlla ˈprɔːprja tʃitˈta}}
4 {{

IPA|ˈaŋke dʒuˈzɛppe ke ˈeːra dɛlla ˈkaːza e dɛlla faˈmiʎʎa di ˈdaːvide dalla tʃitˈta di ˈnaddzaret e dalla ɡaliˈleːa si reˈkɔ in dʒuˈdeːa nɛlla tʃitˈta di ˈdaːvide kjaˈmaːta beˈtlɛmme}}
5 {{

IPA|per ˈfarsi redʒiˈstraːre inˈsjeːme a maˈriːa swa ˈspoːza ke ˈeːra inˈtʃinta}}
6 {{

IPA|ˈprɔːprjo ˈmentre si troˈvaːvano ˈli ˈvɛnne il ˈtempo per ˈlɛːi di partoˈriːre}}
7 {{

IPA|ˈmiːze al ˈmondo il swo primoˈdʒeːnito, lo avˈvɔlse iɱ ˈfaːʃe e lo deˈpoːze in ˈuːna mandʒaˈtɔːja poiˈke non ˈtʃeːra ˈpɔsto per ˈloːro nɛlla loˈkanda]}}

The Standard Italian pronunciation of the text is:

2:1 {{

IPA|[iŋ ˈkwei ˈdʒorni un deˈkreːto di ˈtʃeːzare auˈɡusto ordiˈnaːva ke ssi faˈtʃesse un tʃensiˈmento di ˈtutta la ˈtɛrra}}
2 {{

IPA|ˈkwesto ˈpriːmo tʃensiˈmento fu fˈfatto ˈkwando kwiˈriːno ˈɛːra ɡovernaˈtoːre della ˈsiːrja}}
3 {{

IPA|ˈtutti anˈdaːvano a fˈfarsi redʒiˈstraːre tʃaˈskuːno nella ˈprɔːprja tʃitˈta}}
4 {{

IPA|ˈaŋke dʒuˈzɛppe ke ˈɛːra della ˈkaːsa e ddella faˈmiʎʎa di ˈdaːvide dalla tʃitˈta ddi ˈnaddzaret e ddalla ɡaliˈlɛːa si reˈkɔ in dʒuˈdɛːa nella tʃitˈta ddi ˈdaːvide kjaˈmaːta beˈtlɛmme}}
5 {{

IPA|per ˈfarsi redʒiˈstraːre inˈsjɛːme a mmaˈriːa ˈsa ˈspɔːza ke ˈɛːra inˈtʃinta}}
6 {{

IPA|ˈprɔːprjo ˈmentre si troˈvaːvano ˈli ˈvenne il ˈtɛmpo per ˈlɛi di partoˈriːre}}
7 {{

IPA|ˈmiːse al ˈmondo il ˈso primoˈdʒɛːnito, lo avˈvɔlse iɱ ˈfaʃʃe e llo deˈpoːse in ˈuːna mandʒaˈtoːja poiˈke nnon ˈtʃɛːra ˈposto per ˈloːro nella loˈkanda]}}

See also

  • Italian orthography
  • Italian language
  • Italian grammar
  • Syntactic gemination
  • Wikipedia help page for IPA for Italian - includes English approximations
  • Italian pronunciation guide at Wiktionary

References

1. ^"(...) in a large number of Italian accents, there is considerable friction involved in the pronunciation of {{IPA|[ʎ]}}, creating a voiced palatal lateral fricative (for which there is no established IPA symbol)" {{Harvcoltxt|Ashby|2011|p=64}}.
2. ^Luciano Canepari, A Handbook of Pronunciation, chapter 3: «Italian».
3. ^Romano, Antonio. "A preliminary contribution to the study of phonetic variation of {{IPA|/r/}} in Italian and Italo-Romance." Rhotics. New data and perspectives (Proc. of’r-atics-3, Libera Università di Bolzano (2011): 209-226, pp. 213-214.
4. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=30356&r=69940 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
5. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=30351&r=13567 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
6. ^Luciano Canepari (2012-2015), Dizionario di Pronuncia Italiana Online (DiPI Online), entry gio’.
7. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=76565&r=93607 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
8. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=54775&r=12415 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
9. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=52079&r=40301 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
10. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.dizionario.rai.it/poplemma.aspx?lid=29408&r=10911 | title=Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia}}
11. ^{{Cite web | url=http://www.treccani.it/enciclopedia/sibilanti_(Enciclopedia-dell%27Italiano)/ | title=Sibilanti in "Enciclopedia dell'Italiano"}}
12. ^ Luciano Canepari, A Handbook of Pronunciation, Chapter 3: "Italian", pp. 135-36
13. ^"acoustic data confirm the fact that {{IPA|[|s'tV] /|s'tV/}} (after a pause, or 'silence') is part of the same syllable (a little particular, possibly, on the scale of syllabicity, but nothing really surprising) whereas, obviously, {{IPA|[Vs'tV] /Vs'tV/}} constitute two phono-syllables bordering between two C" Luciano Canepari, A Handbook of Pronunciation, Chapter 3: "Italian", p. 136.
14. ^ "Stress in Italian occurs most often on the penultimate syllable (paroxytones); it also occurs on the antepenultimate syllable (proparoxytones) ...{{Harvcoltxt|Borrelli|2002|p=8}}.

Bibliography

{{refbegin}}
  • {{citation

|last=Ashby
|first=Patricia
|title=Understanding Phonetics
|year=2011
|publisher=Routledge
|series=Understanding Language series
|isbn=978-0340928271
}}
  • {{cite journal

|last=Bertinetto
|first=Pier Marco
|last2=Loporcaro
|first2=Michele
|year=2005
|title=The sound pattern of Standard Italian, as compared with the varieties spoken in Florence, Milan and Rome
|journal=Journal of the International Phonetic Association
|volume=35
|issue=2
|pages=131–151
|doi=10.1017/S0025100305002148
|ref=harv
|url=https://www.zora.uzh.ch/33383/1/S0025100305002148a.pdf
}}
  • {{citation

|last=Borrelli
|first=Doris
|year=2002
|title=Raddoppiamento Sintattico in Italian: A Synchronic and Diachronic Cross-Dialectical Study
|series=Outstanding dissertations in linguistics
|volume=0
|publisher=Psychology Press
|place=New York
|isbn=978-0415942072
}}
  • {{citation

|last=Canepari
|first=Luciano
|year=1992
|title=Il MªPi – Manuale di pronuncia italiana
|trans-title=Handbook of Italian Pronunciation
|language=Italian
|location=Bologna
|publisher=Zanichelli
|isbn=978-88-08-24624-0
}}
  • {{cite journal

|last=Cossu
|first=Giuseppe
|last2=Shankweiler
|first2=Donald
|last3=Liberman
|first3=Isabelle Y.
|last4=Katz
|first4=Leonard
|last5=Tola
|first5=Giuseppe
|title=Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children
|journal=Applied Psycholinguistics
|year=1988
|volume=9
|issue=1
|pages=1–16
|doi=10.1017/S0142716400000424
|ref=harv
}}
  • {{cite web

|last=Costamagna
|first=Lidia
|title=The acquisition of Italian L2 affricates: The case of a Brazilian learner
|url=http://www.nupffale.ufsc.br/newsounds/Papers/11.Costamagna_Lidia.pdf
|work=New Sounds: Proceedings of the Fifth International Symposium on the Acquisition of Second Language Speech
|pages=138–148
|year=2007
|ref=harv
}}
  • {{cite journal

|last=D'Imperio
|first=Mariapaola
|title=Phonetics and phonology of main stress in Italian
|journal=Phonology
|volume=16
|issue=1
|last2=Rosenthall
|first2=Sam
|pages=1–28
|year=1999
|ref=harv
|jstor=4420141
|doi=10.1017/S0952675799003681
  • {{cite journal

|last=Fasolo
|first=Mirco
|last2=Majorano
|first2=Marinella
|last3=D'Odorico
|first3=Laura
|title=Babbling and first words in children with slow expressive development
|journal=Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics
|year=2006
|volume=22
|issue=2
|pages=83–94
|doi=10.1080/02699200701600015
|pmid=17896213
|ref=harv
|hdl=10281/2219
  • {{cite journal

|last=Hall
|first=Robert A. Jr.
|year=1944
|title=Italian phonemes and orthography
|journal=Italica
|volume=21
|issue=2
|pages=72–82
|doi=10.2307/475860
|jstor=475860
|ref=harv
}}
  • {{cite journal

|last=Keren-Portnoy
|first=Tamar
|last2=Majorano
|first2=Marinella
|last3=Vihman
|first3=Marilyn M.
|title=From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian*
|journal=Journal of Child Language
|year=2009
|volume=36
|issue=2
|pages=235–267
|doi=10.1017/S0305000908008933
|ref=harv
|pmid=18789180
}}
  • {{SOWL}}
  • {{cite journal

|last=Majorano
|first=Marinella
|last2=D'Odorico
|first2=Laura
|title=The transition into ambient language: A longitudinal study of babbling and first word production of Italian children
|journal=First Language
|year=2011
|volume=31
|issue=1
|pages=47–66
|doi=10.1177/0142723709359239
|ref=harv
}}
  • {{Citation

|last=Recasens
|first=Daniel
|year=2013
|title=On the articulatory classification of (alveolo)palatal consonants
|journal=Journal of the International Phonetic Association
|volume=43
|issue=1
|pages=1–22
|url=http://pagines.uab.cat/danielrecasens/sites/pagines.uab.cat.danielrecasens/files/Alveolopalatals%20JIPA.pdf
|doi=10.1017/S0025100312000199
}}
  • {{cite journal

|last = Rogers
|first= Derek
|last2 = d'Arcangeli
|first2 = Luciana
|year= 2004
|title=Italian
|journal=Journal of the International Phonetic Association
|volume=34
|issue=1
|pages=117–121
|doi = 10.1017/S0025100304001628
|ref=harv
|url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/services/aop-cambridge-core/content/view/426259D72EEC9F71664AC2F2D3A0FD30/S0025100304001628a.pdf/italian.pdf
}}
  • {{cite web

|last=Zmarich
|first=Claudio
|title=Phonetic Inventories in Italian Children aged 18-27 months: a Longitudinal Study
|url=http://www.aisv.it/AISVScuolaEstiva2006/materials/C.Zmarich/Zmarich&Bonifacio%20(2005).pdf
|work=Interspeech
|last2=Bonifacio
|first2=Serena
|pages=757–760
|year=2005
|ref=harv
}}{{refend}}

External links

  • The online edition (2007) of the Dizionario d'ortografia e di pronunzia (DOP), a pronouncing dictionary of standard Italian
{{Language phonologies}}{{DEFAULTSORT:Italian Phonology}}WP:IPA for Italian

2 : Italian language|Language phonologies

随便看

 

开放百科全书收录14589846条英语、德语、日语等多语种百科知识,基本涵盖了大多数领域的百科知识,是一部内容自由、开放的电子版国际百科全书。

 

Copyright © 2023 OENC.NET All Rights Reserved
京ICP备2021023879号 更新时间:2024/11/12 5:52:10